Skip to main content

Exploring the directionality in the relationship between descriptive and injunctive parental and peer norms and snacking behavior in a three-year-cross-lagged study

Abstract

Background

People’s eating behavior is assumed to be influenced by what other people do (perceived descriptive norms) and what others approve of (perceived injunctive norms). It has been suggested that adolescents are more susceptible to peer norms than parental norms, because they experience a strong need for group acceptance that leads to conforming to peer group norms. The current study examined changes in snacking behavior and four types of social norms (i.e., parental and peer descriptive and injunctive norms) that promoted fruit and vegetable intake among adolescents. This study was the first to examine whether snacking behavior also influenced norm perceptions by testing the directionality of these associations.

Methods

The study consisted of 819 participants (M [SD] age = 11.19 [1.36]; 46.1% boys), collected at three time points (T1 = 2016, T2 = 2017 and T3 = 2018) during the MyMovez project. Self-reported frequency of snack consumption, perceived parental and peer descriptive and injunctive norms were assessed. The primary analysis consisted of a series of cross-lagged autoregressive models specified in a structural equation modeling framework.

Results

Model comparisons testing the descriptive and injunctive norms in separate models and in an additional combined model revealed evidence for bi-directional associations between norms and snacking behavior. Descriptive peer and parent norms were not found to have an effect on subsequent snacking behaviors. Perceived injunctive parental norms were positively associated with healthy snack food intake and negatively associated with unhealthy snack intake (forward direction). Injunctive peer norms were negatively associated with healthy snack food intake. In addition, higher unhealthy snack food intake was negatively associated with the perception of descriptive and injunctive parental norms 1 year later (reversed direction). We did not find peer norms to be more closely associated with changes in snacking behaviors compared to parental norms.

Conclusions

Parents expecting their children to snack healthy had a positive influence on healthy snacking behavior whereas only acting as a healthy role model did not. Future research should address the possible interaction between descriptive and injunctive norms. Research should also take into account the bi-directional relations between eating behaviors and normative perceptions.

Introduction

The prevalence of overweight and obesity is still rising among school-aged children in Western countries [1, 2]. Given that eating habits persist in adulthood, the importance of establishing healthy eating practices during childhood, and maintaining them throughout adolescence, has been acknowledged for a long time [3, 4]. Eating a sufficient amount of fruits and vegetables is an important factor in preventing obesity and avoiding chronic diseases [5, 6]. Even so, most adolescents dislike fruit and vegetables and prefer high-fat and high-sugary ‘fast-food-style’ products [7, 8]. Research has shown that their (snack) food intake is strongly influenced by close social connections [9], which provides opportunities for prevention. The development of effective strategies to improve adolescents’ eating behaviors requires an understanding of how significant others (i.e., parents and peers) influence eating behavior [10]. In addition, it is important to examine whether adolescents’ own eating behavior influences their perception of what close others do and approve of. This study is the first study that explored this bi-directionality between parental and peer social norms and snack food intake among adolescents.

People tend to conform to other persons’ consumption behavior in various social contexts [11]. Specifically, empirical and cross-sectional studies have shown that people’s consumption behavior is influenced by descriptive and injunctive social norms. Perceived descriptive norms are informational non-coercive guidelines that people conform to, whereas injunctive norms exert pressure on one’s behavior and relate to the feeling of external expectations and (dis)approval of ‘(un)appropriate’ eating behavior [12]. These social norms have been identified as powerful mechanisms in determining adolescents’ and adults’ consumption behavior [11, 13,14,15,16,17,18]. Although the research field on social norms in adolescents is still evolving, few experimental normative studies have already shown promising findings to promote healthy consumption behavior [10]. For example, an intervention study successfully targeted water drinking among schoolchildren by influencing the peer group norm [19]. That is, selected peers were secretly instructed to promote water drinking by increasing their own water drinking behavior and talking about the benefits of water drinking to others, which increased water drinking and decreased the consumption of sugar-sweetened beverages among their class mates. Other experimental studies found that adolescents increased their vegetable intake after exposing them to information about the amount that their peers ate [20, 21]. In addition, an online study found that an injunctive peer norm promoting fruit and vegetable intake was associated with higher self-reported fruit and vegetable intake as well as lower unhealthy snack food intake [18]. These findings indicate that both descriptive and injunctive norms influence adolescents’ healthy consumption behavior. Notably, these perceived norms are typically assumed to affect subsequent eating behavior. The reversed impact of eating behaviors on perceived norms has yet to be empirically tested. Therefore, we also examined whether adolescents’ own eating behavior affected their perception of the social norm in a three-wave longitudinal study.

Although young people, and adolescents especially, are influenced by their peers’ consumption behavior [11, 16, 22], their parents also represent important figures in forming dietary patterns by serving as nutritional gatekeepers and role models [8, 23,24,25,26]. It has been suggested that parental influences decline when competing with peer influences, because adolescents experience a strong need for group acceptance that leads to conforming to normative behavior of peers [15, 16, 27,28,29]. Studies have found that even if adolescents have appropriate nutritional knowledge, they still choose fast-food-style products within their school and social environment because they think it is emotionally and socially too risky (e.g., ‘not cool’) to show interest in healthy eating [7, 16]. To our knowledge, only one cross-sectional study has examined both parental and peer descriptive and injunctive norms among adolescents. The study showed that only descriptive parental norms influenced self-reported fruit and vegetable consumption [30], which did not support literature suggesting that peer norms would be more important than parental norms. Notably, they also found negative correlations between parental as well as peer injunctive norms and healthy eating behavior, which could suggest potential ‘reactance effects’ [31]. Reactance effects were also found in previous studies on injunctive norms and eating behavior among adults, especially [32, 33]. It has been suggested that the pressure from an injunctive norm may lead to a dismissal or even backfiring of the intended effect, because people feel pressured or threatened in their sense of freedom [31].

Surprisingly, longitudinal research investigating the relative importance of parental and peer influences on eating behavior is scarce [34,35,36,37]. A study that examined adolescents’ intentions and self-reported fruit, vegetable and water consumption by integrating antecedents from different theoretical approaches (i.e., theory of planned behavior, social norms, and intrinsic motivation) in one model, found that perceived descriptive parental norms (and not injunctive nor any peer norms) predicted behavioral change on water drinking only [37]. Furthermore, a longitudinal study among a student population that focused merely on descriptive peer norms showed no evidence of descriptive norms being a reliable predictor of future snacking and drinking behavior. That is, perceptions about how much peers consumed sugary snack foods and (alcoholic) drinks had limited effect on students’ eating and drinking behavior 1 year later [35]. Altogether, more research is needed to determine the directional relationships between both parental and peer descriptive and injunctive norms on the promotion of healthy snacking behavior.

In this study, we not only examined the generally assumed ordering of norms preceding eating behavior (forward), but also considered the possibility of eating behaviors preceding perceived norms (reversed), as well as possible bi-directional associations (reciprocal) between these constructs [36]. For example, when we make assumptions about how much fruits and vegetables other people consume (i.e., descriptive norm), this perception could be biased by our own eating behavior. Notably, it also provides a different explanation to what is now assumed to be reactance to injunctive norms. It may be that when people usually do not eat a lot of fruits and vegetables - and are aware of this -, they assume and perceive others thinking that they should eat fruit and vegetables. So, one’s own eating behavior may, in fact, influence our perceptions of what others expect from us. In addition, social norms and snacking behavior may be related in a bi-directional manner in which the above mentioned processes are combined into positive and/or negative feedback loops.

The current study is the first to examine changes in snacking behavior and four types of social norms that promoted fruit and vegetable intake, and the directionality of these associations among school-aged adolescents by applying cross-lagged autoregressive models to a three-wave longitudinal study. To fully understand the potential impact of norms on snacking behavior and vice versa, we included fruit and vegetable (‘core’) snack food intake as well as sweet and savory (‘non-core’) snack food intake in our study [38, 39]. We took a step-wise approach by comparing four structural models to investigate the proposed cross-lagged effects for descriptive and injunctive norms separately. We had the following set of research questions (belonging to each step and model) to investigate the directionality between norms and behavior:

  • RQ1. To what extent are social norms and snacking behaviors stable from T1 to T2, and T2 to T3 (i.e., baseline or stability model)?

  • RQ2. Do parental and peer social norms at T1 predict snacking behaviors at T2, and from T2 to T3 (i.e., forward model)?

  • RQ3. Do core and non-core snacking behaviors predict parental and peer social norms from T1 to T2, and T2 to T3 (i.e., reversed model)?

  • RQ4. Are there bidirectional relationships between social norms and snacking behaviors (i.e., reciprocal model)?

Methods

Participants and procedure

Participants were recruited into a large-scale cross-sequential cohort study through their schools (21 primary (n = 453) and secondary (n = 500) schools) in the Netherlands as part of the so-called MyMovez project [40]. All (sub)urban schools following a regular education program were eligible for participation ranging from primary school level, and lower vocational training to secondary school. All schools located near the Nijmegen area were invited to participate in the MyMovez project. Further, Dutch Public Health Services (GGD) of each province were contacted to promote the MyMovez project at their regions. In addition, schools were invited to participate via personal contacts of students and researchers. This random procedure resulted in the participation of 21 (sub)urban schools throughout the Netherlands. Active written consent was obtained from the school directors, caretakers and the participants themselves. Participants received a smartphone with the MyMovez research application for nearly a week. They received random invitations to fill out questionnaires on their smartphone each day between 7:00 AM and 7:30 PM (but not during school hours, except for school breaks) (for detailed information about the recruitment procedure and the Wearable Lab, see [40]). Background and precursor variables were assessed during Phase I of the MyMovez project [40]. For the present study, we made use of data on frequency of snack consumption, perceived descriptive and injunctive parent and peer norms, and several background variables collected over a period of 3 years.

Data collection took place around February and March 2016 (T1), 2017 (T2) and 2018 (T3). The participants were allowed to enter and drop out when they wanted (also during a measurement period), because participation was voluntary. In addition, the participants attending the highest grade levels dropped out at T2 and T3, because they left school. From the 953 participants who initially had parental consent, the response rate was 71% at T1, 40% at T2 and 33% at T3. Participants who answered to the set of questions used for this study at least once were included in the study (N = 819; 47.5% primary school children; 46.1% boys; M(SD) age = 11.19 (1.36); > 90% Dutch origin).

Measures

Core and non-core snack intake

Participant’s self-reported snack consumption was assessed by items from a food frequency questionnaire (FFQ) which accounted for Dutch food items based on the Dutch EPIC FFQ [41]. Participants were asked to recall every other day (i.e., three times per data wave on 2 weekdays and a weekend day)Footnote 1 how many pieces of snack food items they consumed on the previous day with answering options ranging from 0 = none to 6 = six or more. Examples of frequently eaten snack food items were given after each question to further explain the items to the participants. The number of units were multiplied by the average kilo caloric value representing each food item category to place relative weight on the food items (e.g., a small cookie does not equal the energy value of a piece of pie).Footnote 2 The participant’s reported consumption was averaged for each food item per wave. Next, a clear distinction was made between core and non-core items based on previous literature and nutritional guidelines of the Dutch Nutrition Centre [38, 39, 42]. This resulted in three snack food item categories: 1) core: fruit and vegetables; 2) non-core: small, large and wrapped cookies, sweet pastry, chocolates, chocolate bars, candy and liquorice, savory and warm (pastry) snacks, chips, ice cream, and 3) an in-between category: (skimmed) milk, cottage cheese, nuts. A proportional snack intake value was then calculated by adding up the three categories for a total snack consumption score and dividing the kilo caloric values for core and non-core snacks by the total snack consumption score. The two core and non-core measures were used in the analyses.

Descriptive norms

Perceived descriptive norms on fruit and vegetable snack intake were assessed with two separate items about parents and friends: ‘How often do your parents/friends eat fruits and vegetables as a snack?’ [30]. Response items ranged from 1 (‘never’) – 6 (‘always’).

Injunctive norms

Participants’ perceptions of social pressure on their fruit and vegetable snack intake were assessed with two separate items for parents and friends: ‘Do you think that your parents/friends believe you should eat fruits and vegetables as a snack?’ [30]. Response options ranged from 1 (‘no, certainly do not’) – 6 (‘yes, certainly do’).

Covariates

Age, sex and weight status were mentioned in empirical studies as potential confounders in the relationship between social norms and snack intake [11, 15, 43]. Demographic variables age and sex were supplied by the schools’ administration offices. Weight and length was measured each year by trained researchers following standard procedures. Standardized BMI scores were calculated accounting for variations in growth curves of children and adolescents [44].

Strategy of analyses

Descriptive statistics were calculated to examine the distribution (minimum, maximum and means) of all model items and the differences between time points T1-T3. In addition, bivariate correlations among all model items were computed.

The primary analyses consisted of a series of competing cross-lagged autoregressive models specified in a structural equation modeling framework in Mplus version 7.2 [45, 46]. To examine our research questions, 2 sets of four models were compared for descriptive and injunctive norms on snack intake. Model 1 (answering RQ1) was the baseline or stability model, which only included autoregressive paths (estimating intra-individual stability) between the three assessments and all concurrent correlations among constructs. Model 2 (RQ2) examined social norms as predictors of snacking behaviors, and included the same parameters as Model 1, but also included cross-lagged paths from T1 and T2 social norms to T2 and T3 snack intake, respectively. Model 3 (RQ3) examined snacking behaviors as predictors of social norms, and included the same parameters as Model 1, but also included cross-lagged paths from T1 and T2 snack intake to T2 and T3 social norms, respectively. Model 4 (RQ4) examined bidirectional associations between social norms and snacking behaviors, and included all parameters specified in Models 1, 2 and 3. Figure 1 presents an overview of the four models. Age, sex, and BMI were included as covariates in the models on T1 based on their significant correlations with the model variables under investigation (see Table 2). The parameters in the models were estimated using (Full-Information) Maximum Likelihood estimation with robust standard errors (MLR in Mplus) to account for missing values and potential deviations from multivariate normality. In the additional analysis, we combined all variables (i.e., descriptive and injunctive parent and peer norms) in one model to examine the bi-directional relationship between norms and snacking behavior.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Baseline, forward, reversed and reciprocal models for social norms and snacking behavior

For each model, model fit information was assessed by the following fit indices: the χ2 test of model fit, the Standardized Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA; satisfactory values below .06) [47], the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) and the Tucker-Lewis Index (cut-off values close to or above .90). Next, Chi-square difference tests with MLR scaling correction were assessed to assess which model(s) provided a significantly better fit to the data.

Results

Descriptive statistics

The distribution of study variables are presented in Table 1 and the correlations between all model variables including covariates are presented in Table 2. The difference tests displayed in Table 1 show that all study variables were stable over time, except for a statistically significant decrease in non-core snack intake between T1 and T2 only (p = .002). Table 2 shows cross-sectional correlations and there were significant positive correlations between descriptive parental norms and core snack intake across T1, T2 and T3, and negative correlations between descriptive parental norms and non-core food intake (all p-values <.05). Injunctive peer norms and non-core food intake were negatively correlated at T1 (r = −.09, p = .02) and T2 (r = −.13. p = .03). There were almost no significant correlations (all p-values >.05) between descriptive and injunctive peer norms and core snack intake (except for T2 injunctive peer norm and core snack intake (r = .13, p = .03).

Table 1 Distribution of study variables. MyMovez study, 2016, 2017 and 2018
Table 2 Correlations between all model variables. MyMovez study, 2016, 2017 and 2018

The bivariate correlations in Table 2 also showed that several descriptive and injunctive norms were correlated (e.g., descriptive peer norm with descriptive parental norm on T1, T2 and T3). Therefore, in the additional analysis, we explored whether combining the influence of all norms and snacking behavior in one model would lead to different findings than the planned main analyses.

Main analyses

Table 3 provides an overview of goodness of fit indices for all four structural path models for descriptive and injunctive norms, separately. All models provided acceptable fit to the observed data with all Chi-Square test of model fits being significant (p-values < .01) and according to the values for RMSEA (.028–.032), CFI (.942–.978) and TLI (.942–.956).

Table 3 Goodness of fit statistics for the tested models on social norms and snacking behavior. MyMovez study (N = 819), 2016, 2017 and 2018

Descriptive norms

The chi-square difference tests between the models for descriptive norms showed that Models 2, 3 and 4 did not provide better insight than the baseline Model 1. This means that social norms and behaviors were stable over time (RQ1), and that there were no uni- or bi-directional relationships between parental and peer descriptive norms and snacking behavior (RQ2–4). Table 4 presents the model findings of Model 1 with the auto-regressive effects and cross-sectional correlations for descriptive norms.

Table 4 Standardized Estimates for Model 1 and Model 4 on descriptive and injunctive parental and peer norms and snacking behavior

The cross-sectional findings show that perceived descriptive parental norms were negatively correlated with non-core food intake on T1 and T2 (p-values < .01), but not on T3. Descriptive parental norms and core food intake were positively correlated on T1 only (p < .001). Notably, the correlations on T2 and T3 were not significant compared to the bivariate correlations (without missing value imputations) presented in Table 2. Perceived descriptive peer norms were only positively correlated with core food intake on T1 (p < .05). Perceived descriptive peer norms were negatively correlated with non-core food intake on T1 and T2 (p < .001 and p < .05, respectively), but not on T3. In addition, peer norms were positively correlated with descriptive parental norms on T1, T2 and T3 (p-values < .001).

Injunctive norms

The chi-square difference tests between the models for injunctive norms show that both Model 2 (RQ2 - forward) and 4 (RQ4 - reciprocal) fit the data better than Model 1 (p = .024 and p = .032, respectively), and that Model 4 fitted the data better than Model 3 (p = .025). Comparing the sample-size adjusted BIC’s between Model 1 and 2 (diff = 11), and Model 1 and 4 (diff = 28), there is stronger evidence that Model 4 provides better insight into the relationship between injunctive norms and snacking behavior, suggesting a bi-directional relationship between injunctive norms and behavior. Figure 2 depicts the findings of Model 4 for injunctive norms (only marginal and statistical significant paths are shown). Table 4 presents also the cross-sectional correlations and control variables.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Model 4 for injunctive norms and snacking behavior only (above) and Model 4 for descriptive and injunctive norms in one model (below)

Figure 2 shows that the injunctive peer norm was negatively associated with core snack intake from T1 to T2 (p = .04), but not from T2 to T3 (p = .55), and that the parental injunctive norm was positively associated with core snack intake from T1 to T2 (p = .003), but not from T2 to T3 (p = .30). From T2 to T3, the model shows that there is a significant negative effect of the injunctive parental norm on non-core food intake (β = −.21, p = .033) and a marginal reversed effect of non-core food intake on injunctive parental norms (β = −.19, p = .087). Non-core food intake was not associated with injunctive peer norms over time. These results suggest that perceived parental pressure to snack healthy can influence snacking behavior of adolescents by affecting core snack intake positively and non-core snack intake negatively over time. In addition, the model provides the first evidence of potential cross-lagged associations between non-core snack food intake and injunctive parental norms. That is, that when adolescents snack unhealthy, they may perceive less pressure of their parents to snack healthy.

The cross-sectional findings resemble the correlations (without missing value imputations) presented in Table 2. They show that injunctive parental norms and core food intake were positively correlated on T1 (p < .001) and marginally significant on T2 (p = .072). Injunctive peer norms were not correlated with core food intake on T1, T2 and T3. Injunctive parental norms and non-core food intake was only negatively correlated on T1 (p < .001) and marginally on T2 (p = .064), and injunctive peer norms was also negatively correlated with non-core food intake on T1 only (p < .01). In addition, injunctive peer norms were positively correlated with parental injunctive norms on T1, T2 and T3 (p-values < .001).

Additional analyses

As additional analyses, we entered the four norm variables with snacking behavior in one model and compared each of the four models (Model 1 - Model 4) again. All four models provided acceptable fit to the observed data with all Chi-Square test of model fits being significant (p < .0001) and according to the values for RMSEA (.034–.037), CFI (.937–.944) and TLI (.897–.905) (see Table 3). The chi-square difference tests between the combined normative models show that Model 4 (RQ4 - reciprocal) fits the data better than Model 1 (p = .047). Figure 2 depicts the model findings only showing marginal and statistically significant paths without control variables and cross-sectional correlations, which can be found in Table 4.

Compared to our main analyses (Model 4) testing injunctive norms and snacking behavior only, the combined model depicted in Fig. 2 shows similar findings from T1 to T2; however, the cross-lagged paths disappear from T2 to T3 (all p-values > .10). Interestingly, this model shows reversed paths of non-core snack food intake on parental descriptive norms from T1 to T2 and T2 to T3 (β = −.20, p = .037 and β = −.16, p = .058, respectively). This suggests that adolescents with higher non-core snack food intake report that they do not see their parents consume core snack foods often.

The cross-sectional findings on T1, T2 and T3 are similar to the two separately tested models for descriptive (Model 1) and injunctive norms (Model 4). In line with the correlations in Table 2, there were strong cross-sectional correlations between parental and peer injunctive and descriptive norms (all p-values < .05).

Discussion

The present study examined the temporal sequence between social norms and snack food intake among adolescents by applying cross-lagged autoregressive models to a three-wave longitudinal study. We provide the first evidence that there are bi-directional relationships between norms and behavior. Our findings from the main and additional analyses suggested that behavior may also affect the perception of norms (reversed direction). That is, higher unhealthy snack food intake was negatively associated with the perception of descriptive and injunctive parental norms 1 year later. Further, our study showed that perceived injunctive parental norms were positively associated with healthy snack food intake and negatively associated with unhealthy snack intake (forward direction). Injunctive peer norms were negatively associated with healthy snack food intake. In addition, we found no clear evidence that peer norms could be more important compared to parental norms considering (changes in) snacking behavior.

Most experimental research on social norms and eating behavior seem in favor of using descriptive norms to influence eating behavior because they are found to have greater effect on eating behavior compared to injunctive norms, and may avoid potential reactance effects [48,49,50]. Although we did not manipulate the perceived norms in our study, our findings are important for this field of research when aiming to develop interventions with long term impact [10, 19, 40]. In our study, we did not find an effect over time of perceived peer and parental descriptive norms on (un)healthy snack intake. With regard to peer descriptive norms, this is in line with the only study on longitudinal effects of descriptive norms on snacking behavior among students [35]. Interestingly, the findings from our additional analyses did show a relatively stable reversed effect of unhealthy snacking behavior on the perception of the descriptive parental norm over time; that is, adolescents reporting a relatively higher unhealthy snack intake, reported that they had seen their parents eating healthy snack foods less often in the subsequent years. This may imply that giving the right example as a parent does not necessarily impact their children’s healthy snacking behavior (i.e., modeling); instead, children may use their parents’ unhealthy behavior as an excuse or justification to snack unhealthy. This adds an interesting new dimension to role modeling. Instead of seeing is doing, these findings may suggest that doing is preventing. So, giving the right example as a parent could prevent their children from snacking unhealthy foods as opposed to stimulating children to consume healthy products. Nevertheless, this assumption is based on the adolescents’ reports only. More longitudinal research is needed on parental (un)healthy snacking behavior by also assessing parents’ snack intake together with their children’s snack intake.

With regard to perceived injunctive norms, this study showed that the peer injunctive norm was negatively associated with healthy snack intake 1 year later, which is in line with previous studies that refer to reactance [32, 33]. In contrast, when adolescents perceived pressure from their parents to snack healthy, they reported to eat relatively more healthy snacks and less unhealthy snacks. Similar to our findings on descriptive norms, we also did not find evidence on injunctive norms that peers would have more influence on snacking behavior than parents over time [16, 29]. These findings are supported by a study examining the importance of different sources of influence among adolescents, in which participants reported that their parents required them to eat healthy foods more often than their friends, teachers and promotion materials from health authorities [51]. Parents were even perceived as being the most effective source in encouraging them to eat healthy foods [51]. Nevertheless, we should be cautious in giving meaning to these findings as they were not consistent across each time point. Additionally, research on parental feeding styles shows that parental pressure to eat could lead to unhealthy eating practices and discouragement of healthy food intake [52, 53]. It may be that the parent injunctive norm taps into a different kind of pressure than parental feeding styles that are measured, for example, by the Child Feeding Questionnaire (CFQ) [54]. Thus other forms of long-lasting interpersonal influence than social norms may play a more prominent role or interact with norms in explaining changes in snacking behavior during this developmental period. Future research should assess other types of social influences and forms of encouragement such as social support, encouragement or sabotage. In addition, it would be interesting to investigate by use of qualitative research what these type of behaviors or influences are and how adolescents know what their parents and peers think they should do.

In our main analyses testing injunctive norms only, we found a cross-lagged pattern between injunctive parental norms and adolescents reporting to have a relatively lower unhealthy snack intake, and in turn, they reported to perceive less parental pressure a year later. Nevertheless, these effects were not apparent anymore when including descriptive norms. Further, a reversed effect of unhealthy snacking on the descriptive parent norm was found in the additional analyses. Although descriptive and injunctive norms are typically treated as independent predictors of behavior, we suggest that the two type of norms are not mutually exclusive and may interact with each other [55]. Our study supports this notion because the descriptive and injunctive norms were highly correlated. It could imply, for example, that injunctive norms may surpass the influence of descriptive norms because adolescents could have used the descriptive norms as indicator of injunctive norms. In addition, most people know others approve of eating fruit and vegetables even if they or others do not eat healthy themselves. Future research should address the possible interaction between these type of norms and researchers are encouraged to investigate the use of social norms in combined designs to advance the research field on social norms and food intake. For example, interventions that make use of peer behaviors to influence a targeted behavior (such as social network interventions) may benefit from including elements tapping into the use of injunctive norms while this behavior could be perceived as a descriptive norm. In this way, it does not elicit a reactance effect on injunctive norms.

Our study had limitations that should be acknowledged. First, the data relies on self-reported recall of snack food intake and perceptions of social norms which may raise concerns about social desirable answers and participation bias. We tried to reduce potential problems related to these concerns by stressing the anonymous data handling to caregivers and participants, and data were assessed during multiple time points. Second, the accuracy of adolescents’ self-report and how they perceive the snacking behavior of their parents may have changed over the years. Third, the precision of our snacking measures and norms could be improved upon. Future research could profit from assessing eating behavior from multiple sources (e.g., by including parent reports). In addition, we assessed norms for parents and peers in general while it is likely that opinions, norms and eating behaviors differ between parents and various friends. Likewise, future research could make a distinction in whether the snack measures are assessed during weekdays or weekend days when adolescents spend more time with their peers or parents. We also used single items to measure social norms on healthy food intake to not overburden the participants in the MyMovez project. Future studies are advised to use multiple item measures to improve study reliability, and to assess social norms on unhealthy foods as predictors of snacking (un)healthy foods. Investigating the combination of both healthy and unhealthy social norm perceptions would improve insights in social influences on eating behavior. Fourth, majority of the study sample consisted of non-overweight and few overweight or obese participants. It would be interesting to examine a larger sample of overweight or obese participants to investigate their norm perceptions and snacking behaviors.

In conclusion, this was the first study that investigated the directional nature of the relations between descriptive and injunctive parent and peer norms and snack food intake, which has important theoretical and practical implications. First, we suggest that future social norm studies take into account the bi-directional relationship between norms and behavior in their design and when interpreting their findings. Norms do not only influence our eating behavior, but our eating behavior also influences how we perceive norms. Second, we found that parents expecting their children to snack healthy had a positive influence on healthy snacking behavior whereas only acting as a healthy role model did not. We also suggest that previous recommendations on the use of the descriptive norm type for (long-term) behavior change should be tempered, because our study showed that descriptive and injunctive norms are highly correlated and injunctive norms seem to have a stronger impact than descriptive norms. More research is needed to unravel whether people really perceive descriptive norms as intended or whether they may use it as an indicator of injunctive norms, or vice versa. We also emphasize that further longitudinal work is needed to replicate our findings. The development of effective strategies to improve young people’s eating behaviors requires a better understanding of how social connections influence their eating behavior and vice versa.

Availability of data and materials

Data and materials of the MyMovez project are available in the repository of the Radboud University.

The specific datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Notes

  1. T3 also served as a baseline measurement for the MyMovez intervention at the end of the project during Phase II. In order to schedule all required questionnaires without overburdening the participants, the FFQ was assessed once.

  2. Three members of the MyMovez team independently scored kilo caloric values for each of the food items. Inter-coder reliability was consistently high (ĸ = .97).

References

  1. Lobstein T, Baur L, Uauy R. Obesity in children and young people: a crisis in public health. Obes Rev. 2004;5(s1):4–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. World Health Organization. World Health Organization obesity and overweight fact sheet. http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs311/en/. Accessed 16 Aug 2019.

  3. Patterson E, Wärnberg J, Kearney J, Sjöström M. The tracking of dietary intakes of children and adolescents in Sweden over six years: the European youth heart study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2009;6(1):91.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Lake AA, Rugg-Gunn AJ, Hyland RM, Wood CE, Mathers JC, Adamson AJ. Longitudinal dietary change from adolescence to adulthood: perceptions, attributions and evidence. Appetite. 2004;42(3):255–63.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Kim SA, Moore LV, Galuska D, Wright AP, Harris D, Grummer-Strawn LM, et al. Vital signs: fruit and vegetable intake among children-United States, 2003-2010. MMWR Morb Mortal Wkly Rep. 2014;63(31):671–6.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Oyebode O, Gordon-Dseagu V, Walker A, Mindell JS. Fruit and vegetable consumption and all-cause, cancer and CVD mortality: analysis of health survey for England data. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2014;68(9):856.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Brown K, McIlveen H, Strugnell C. Nutritional awareness and food preferences of young consumers. Nutr Food Sci. 2000;30(5):230–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Warwick J, McIlveen H, Strugnell C. Food choices and the younger generation. J Consum Stud Home Econ. 1997;21(2):141–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Higgs S, Thomas J. Social influences on eating. Curr Opin Behav Sci. 2016;9:1–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Higgs S, Liu J, Collins E, Thomas J. Using social norms to encourage healthier eating. Nutr Bull. 2019;44(1):43–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Cruwys T, Bevelander KE, Hermans RCJ. Social modeling of eating: a review of when and why social influence affects food intake and choice. Appetite. 2015;86:3–18.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Higgs S. Social norms and their influence on eating behaviours. Appetite. 2015;86:38–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Wouters EJ, Larsen JK, Kremers SP, Dagnelie PC, Geenen R. Peer influence on snacking behavior in adolescence. Appetite. 2010;55(1):11–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Perkins JM, Perkins HW, Craig DW. Misperceived norms and personal sugar-sweetened beverage consumption and fruit and vegetable intake among students in the United States. Appetite. 2018;129:82–93.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Bevelander KE, Anschütz DJ, Engels RCME. Social norms in food intake among normal weight and overweight children. Appetite. 2012;58(3):864–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Stead M, McDermott L, MacKintosh AM, Adamson A. Why healthy eating is bad for young people’s health: identity, belonging and food. Soc Sci Med. 2011;72(7):1131–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Bruening M, Eisenberg M, MacLehose R, Nanney MS, Story M, Neumark-Sztainer D. Relationship between Adolescents' and their Friends' eating behaviors: breakfast, fruit, vegetable, whole-grain, and dairy intake. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2012;112(10):1608–13.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Stok FM, de Vet E, de Wit JBF, Luszczynska A, Safron M, de Ridder DTD. The proof is in the eating: subjective peer norms are associated with adolescents’ eating behaviour. Public Health Nutr. 2015;18(6):1044–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Smit CR, de Leeuw RN, Bevelander KE, Burk WJ, Buijzen M. A social network-based intervention stimulating peer influence on children's self-reported water consumption: a randomized control trial. Appetite. 2016;103:294–301.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Sharps M, Robinson E. Encouraging children to eat more fruit and vegetables: Health vs. descriptive social norm-based messages. Appetite. 2016;100:18–25.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Sharps M, Robinson E. Perceived eating norms and vegetable consumption in children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12(1):135.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  22. Bevelander KE, Lichtwarck-Aschoff A, Anschütz DJ, Hermans RCJ, Engels RCME. Imitation of snack food intake among normal-weight and overweight children. Front Psychol. 2013;4:949.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Feunekes G, Stafleu A, De Graaf C, Van Staveren W. Family resemblance in fat intake in the Netherlands. Eur J Clin Nutr. 1997;51(12):793.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Savage JS, Fisher JO, Birch LL. Parental influence on eating behavior: conception to adolescence. J Law Med Ethics. 2007;35(1):22–34.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  25. Young EM, Fors SW. Factors related to the eating habits of students in grades 9–12. J Sch Health. 2001;71(10):483–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Sharps M, Higgs S, Blissett J, Nouwen A, Chechlacz M, Allen HA, et al. Examining evidence for behavioural mimicry of parental eating by adolescent females. An observational study. Appetite. 2015;89:56–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Valkenburg PM, Cantor J. The development of a child into a consumer. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2001;22(1):61–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. James K, J TJ, Kirsty M. What parents think: children and healthy eating. Br Food J. 2006;108(5):413–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  29. Hill AJ. Developmental issues in attitudes to food and diet. Proc Nutr Soc. 2002;61(2):259–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Pedersen S, Grønhøj A, Thøgersen J. Following family or friends. Social norms in adolescent healthy eating. Appetite. 2015;86:54–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Brehm SS, Brehm JW. Psychological reactance: a theory of freedom and control: Academic Press; 2013.

  32. Knowles ES, Linn JA. The promise and future of resistance to persuasion. In: Knowles ES, Linn JA, editors. Resistance and persuasion. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associate, Inc.; 2004. p. 301–10.

  33. Stok FM, De Ridder DT, De Vet E, De Wit JB. Don't tell me what I should do, but what others do: the influence of descriptive and injunctive peer norms on fruit consumption in adolescents. Br J Health Psychol. 2014;19(1):52–64.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Mollen S, Rimal RN, Lapinski MK. What is normative in health communication research on norms? A review and recommendations for future scholarship. Health Commun. 2010;25(6–7):544–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Jones A, Robinson E. The longitudinal associations between perceived descriptive peer norms and eating and drinking behavior: an initial examination in young adults. Front Psychol. 2017;8:2.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Robinson E. Perceived social norms and eating behaviour: an evaluation of studies and future directions. Physiol Behav. 2015;152:397–401.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. Smit CR, de Leeuw RN, Bevelander KE, Burk WJ, Buijs L, van Woudenberg TJ, et al. An integrated model of fruit, vegetable, and water intake in young adolescents. Health Psychol. 2018;37(12):1159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Bell A, Kremer P, Magarey A, Swinburn B. Contribution of ‘noncore’foods and beverages to the energy intake and weight status of Australian children. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2005;59(5):639.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. McGowan L, Croker H, Wardle J, Cooke LJ. Environmental and individual determinants of core and non-core food and drink intake in preschool-aged children in the United Kingdom. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2012;66(3):322.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Bevelander KE, Smit CR, van Woudenberg TJ, Buijs L, Burk WJ, Buijzen M. Youth’s social network structures and peer influences: study protocol MyMovez project–phase I. BMC Public Health. 2018;18(1):504.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  41. Ocke MC, Bueno-de-Mesquita HB, Goddijn HE, Jansen A, Pols MA, van Staveren WA, et al. The Dutch EPIC food frequency questionnaire. I. Description of the questionnaire, and relative validity and reproducibility for food groups. Int J Epidemiol. 1997;26(suppl1):S37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. DutchNutritionCentre. Schijf van Vijf Den Haag: Voedingscentrum; 2018 Available from: https://www.voedingscentrum.nl/nl/gezond-eten-met-de-schijf-van-vijf/wat-staat-er-in-de-vakken-van-de-schijf-van-vijf.aspx.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Bevelander KE, Engels RCME, Anschütz DJ, Wansink B. The effect of an intervention on schoolchildren’s susceptibility to a peer’s candy intake. Eur J Clin Nutr. 2013;67(8):829.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Schönbeck Y, Talma H, van Dommelen P, Bakker B, Buitendijk SE, HiraSing RA, et al. Increase in prevalence of overweight in Dutch children and adolescents: a comparison of Nationwide growth studies in 1980, 1997 and 2009. PLoS One. 2011;6(11):e27608.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  45. Selig JP, Little TD. Autoregressive and cross-lagged panel analysis for longitudinal data; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  46. Muthen L, Muthen B. Mplus statistical software, 1998–2012. Los Angeles: Muthén & Muthén; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  47. Hu LT, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. 1999;6(1):1–55.

  48. Lally P, Bartle N, Wardle J. Social norms and diet in adolescents. Appetite. 2011;57(3):623–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Mollen S, Rimal RN, Ruiter RA, Kok G. Healthy and unhealthy social norms and food selection. Findings from a field-experiment. Appetite. 2013;65:83–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Robinson E, Fleming A, Higgs S. Prompting healthier eating: testing the use of health and social norm based messages. Health Psychol. 2014;33(9):1057.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Chan K, Prendergast G, Grønhøj A, Bech-Larsen T. The role of socializing agents in communicating healthy eating to adolescents: a cross-cultural study. J Int Consum Mark. 2010;23(1):59–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. Clark HR, Goyder E, Bissell P, Blank L, Peters J. How do parents' child-feeding behaviours influence child weight? Implications for childhood obesity policy. J Public Health. 2007;29(2):132–41.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  53. Fisher JO, Mitchell DC, Smiciklas-Wright H, Birch LL. Parental influences on young girls’ fruit and vegetable, micronutrient, and fat intakes. J Am Diet Assoc. 2002;102(1):58–64.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Birch LL, Fisher JO, Grimm-Thomas K, Markey CN, Sawyer R, Johnson SL. Confirmatory factor analysis of the child feeding questionnaire: a measure of parental attitudes, beliefs and practices about child feeding and obesity proneness. Appetite. 2001;36(3):201–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Staunton M, Louis WR, Smith JR, Terry DJ, McDonald RI. How negative descriptive norms for healthy eating undermine the effects of positive injunctive norms. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2014;44(4):319–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

The research was funded by the European Research Council under the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007–2013) / ERC grant agreement n° 617253. The funding body was not involved in the study design, data collection, analysis and interpretation as well as manuscript writing.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KEB, WJB and MB conceptualized and designed the study; KEB, CRS, LB, and TJW collected the data; KEB analyzed the data and wrote the paper; WJB, CRS, TJW, LB and MB critically reviewed the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to K. E. Bevelander.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

This project was conducted according to the guidelines described in the Declaration of Helsinki and all procedures were approved by the ethical committee of the European Research Council (617253) and the Faculty Social Sciences, Radboud University, Nijmegen in the Netherlands (ECSW2014–100614-222). Active written consent was obtained from the school directors, caretakers and the participants themselves.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bevelander, K.E., Burk, W.J., Smit, C.R. et al. Exploring the directionality in the relationship between descriptive and injunctive parental and peer norms and snacking behavior in a three-year-cross-lagged study. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 17, 76 (2020). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12966-020-00977-w

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12966-020-00977-w

Keywords