Skip to main content

DEBATE: Do interventions based on behavioral theory work in the real world?

Abstract

Background

Behavioral scientists suggest that for behavior change interventions to work effectively, and deliver population-level health outcomes, they must be underpinned by behavioral theory. However, despite implementation of such interventions, population levels of both health outcomes and linked behaviors have remained relatively static. We debate the extent to which interventions based on behavioral theory work in the real world to address population health outcomes.

Discussion

Hagger argues there is substantive evidence supporting the efficacy and effectiveness of interventions based on behavioral theory in promoting population-level health behavior change in the ‘real world’. However, large-scale effectiveness trials within existing networks are relatively scarce, and more are needed leveraging insights from implementation science. Importantly, sustained investment in effective behavioral interventions is needed, and behavioral scientists should engage in greater advocacy to persuade gatekeepers to invest in behavioral interventions.

Weed argues there is no evidence to demonstrate behavioral theory interventions are genuinely effective in real world settings in populations that are offered them: they are merely efficacious for those that receive them. Despite behavioral volatility that is a normal part of maintaining steady-state population behavior levels creating the illusion of effectiveness, interventions fail in shifting the curve of population behaviors because they focus on individuals rather than populations.

Hagger responds that behavioral interventions work in the ‘real world’ in spite of, not because of, flux in health behaviors, and that the contention that behavioral theory focuses solely on individual behavior change is inaccurate.

Weed responds that the focus on extending the controls of efficacy trials into implementation is impractical, uneconomic and futile, and this has squandered opportunities to conduct genuine effectiveness trials in naturalistic settings.

Summary

Hagger contends that interventions based on behavioral theory are effective in changing population-level behavior in ‘real world’ contexts, but more evidence on how best to implement them and how to engage policymakers and practitioners to provide sustained funding is needed. Weed argues for a paradigm shift, away from aggregative attempts to effect individual behavior change towards a focus on disrupting social practices, underpinned by understanding social and economic causation of the distribution and acceptance of behaviors in a population.

Introduction

Given evidence for the prevalence and the human and economic burden of non-communicable diseases, behaviors that may contribute to the incidences of such diseases are of increasing academic, political and societal concern. We debate the extent to which interventions based on behavioral theory work in the real world to contribute to addressing these concerns. This arises from a live debate at the Annual Meeting of the International Society of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity held in Hong Kong in June 2018. The debate reported in this article was conducted using the same format as the ‘live’ debate, our cases were each written independently then exchanged simultaneously, and the same process repeated for the responses. Once this process was completed, we authored the joint conclusion comprising points of agreement, and areas where we disagree.

In favor: Martin S. Hagger - interventions based on behavioral theory do work in the real world

Background

Given epidemiological research associating multiple chronic disease risk with participation in health-related behaviors [1,2,3,4,5], health organizations and stakeholders have sought to develop behavioral interventions that lead to practically-significant changes in these behaviors and concomitant reduction in disease risk. Behavioral scientists propose that interventions based on theories from the behavioral sciences, particularly psychology, will be optimally effective in evoking behavior change [6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13]. Despite an expanding evidence base demonstrating the efficacy of theory-based interventions in promoting sustained change in health-related behavior, my colleague will suggest that the role of behavioral theory is overstated, particularly when it comes to ‘real world’ effectiveness. In particular, he will argue that what works in ‘ideal world’ carefully-controlled conditions are not effective in ‘real world’ contexts where upscaling, logistic, cultural, and implementation factors pervade. He will cite the lack of change in physical activity participation and rates of chronic disease and obesity as evidence that interventions based on behavioral theory are not effective. Here I deconstruct these ‘straw person’ arguments, and contend that interventions based on theory can and do work in promoting behavior change in real world contexts.

Do interventions based on theory ‘work’ in changing behavior?

Behavioral theory is a broad term for a set of pre-specified ideas or predictions aimed at explaining behavior [7, 14, 15]. Behavioral theories come from multiple disciplines (e.g., psychology, sociology, behavioral economics), and identify multiple determinants or mechanisms of behavior including beliefs, motivation and intentions [16], individual differences [17], social influence [18], and environment and demographics [19,20,21]. A substantive body of research has identified the effectiveness of theory-based interventions targeting change in modifiable determinants or mechanisms [22,23,24]. For example, syntheses of evidence have indicated that interventions targeting change in social cognitive beliefs and motivation [25,26,27,28,29,30,31], social support and norms [32, 33], and planning [29] to be effective in promoting behavior change in randomized controlled trials. Similarly, interventions based on health-risk communications have been successful in promoting behavior change [34], with graphic images on tobacco products a prominent example [35,36,37,38]. Research targeting change in determinants derived from social-ecological theories, encompassing environmental, community, and policy factors, have also been shown to be effective [21, 32, 39, 40]. Interventions based on choice architecture, sometimes referred to as ‘nudging’, have demonstrated effectiveness in changing behavior in laboratory and field settings [41,42,43,44,45,46]. In addition, interventions adopting specific strategies such as self-monitoring [47, 48], prompting social support [48], planning [49], behavioral skills [31], and affective appeals [50] have been found to be particularly effective. Taken together, primary studies and research syntheses indicate that theory-based interventions are effective in changing behavior in laboratory and ‘real world’ contexts [51].

In the interests of balance, it would be remiss not to acknowledge a number of caveats to this evidence. Meta-analyses and systematic reviews have also indicated that stated theoretical basis leads to no difference in intervention effectiveness [52], and, in some cases, even reverse effects [53]. Similarly, there is research demonstrating that adoption of particular behavior change strategies does not lead to greater intervention effectiveness [52, 54, 55]. So how can these two streams of evidence be reconciled? Inadequate mapping of theory on to intervention components may be a moderating factor. A distinction has been made between theory-inspired and theory-based interventions [56, 57]. Prestwich et al. [52] indicated that ‘theory-inspired’ interventions provide insufficient specification of links between theory and intervention strategies. Theory-inspired interventions, therefore, pay ‘lip service’ to behavioral theory, but fail to link intervention components with relevant theoretical determinants. There are also problems with inadequate reporting of such links, which hinders researchers’ ability to evaluate the effect of theoretical basis on intervention effectiveness. There is therefore a need for researchers to become more effective in matching theoretical determinants of behavior with intervention content, and for greater transparency when reporting intervention content [56, 58].

Why have behavioral interventions not altered the course of non-communicable disease pandemics?

If interventions based on behavioral theory work in changing behavior in ‘real world’ contexts, how have they not stemmed the tide of non-communicable disease pandemics, as my colleague will contend? Knowledge and implementation of effective interventions, whether or not they are based on theory, seems to have had limited impact in changing population-level participation in health behaviors and reducing incidence of chronic disease [13]. Although there is substantive evidence that behavioral interventions are effective in changing behavior across multiple contexts, populations, and behaviors, and, arguably, those based on theory having greater effectiveness despite some of the aforementioned limitations, such knowledge is seldom translated to population-level change. This is largely because many behavioral interventions implemented at the community or even population level are relatively short lived, under-funded, or fail due to poor implementation, up-scaling, or translation [59]. Funding is a key issue; many behavioral interventions receive initial investment that is not sustained [60]. Interventions need sufficient funding for the necessary networks and providers required to implement the intervention in practice. Even though economic evaluation of many behavior change interventions has demonstrated their cost effectiveness [61], investment in behavioral interventions pales compared to investment in procedures aimed at treating disease [60]. It is unrealistic to expect health care providers to identify, assimilate, and implement research findings reported in scientific outlets. The onus is on those producing the evidence to actively engage governments, stakeholders and policymakers, and outline the human and economic advantages of preventive strategies like behavioral interventions over a treatment-focused model of healthcare provision [62].

Related to this, behavioral scientists need to better demonstrate how theory-based behavioral interventions that work in lab and field experiments, and have been shown to be effective in larger randomized controlled trials and in real world contexts, can be implemented in practice [9, 15, 63, 64]. Such evidence should be the focus of evidence presentations to government and policymakers advocating investment in, and implementation of, behavioral interventions [65]. The expanding discipline of implementation science focuses on translation of research findings into evidence-based practice, and is receiving increased attention in the fields of behavioral science, public health, health promotion, and health policy [65,66,67]. In the context of behavioral interventions, implementation science examines the pathways and strategies necessary for the uptake and implementation of interventions by policymakers and providers. Evidence on how behavioral interventions can be developed by key workers within existing networks, who will ultimately be responsible for implementing the intervention (e.g., health ministers, healthcare providers, school administrators and teachers, workplace health managers, community leaders, urban planners), and how users of the intervention (i.e., those whose behavior needs to change) can be involved in the implementation, is important to ensure that interventions are practically relevant and sensitive to the contextual and cultural characteristics of target populations [64, 68]. In addition, research on how theory-based behavioral interventions can be upscaled so their reach within target populations is maximized and the changes in health behavior and health outcomes promised by formative research realized [69]. Research is needed to identify the conditions necessary to up-scale behavioral interventions in real world contexts, including identifying the partnerships needed to fund, implement, monitor, and maintain interventions; engaging stakeholders to assess the feasibility and acceptability of implementing the intervention in the target community or setting [70]; assisting governmental agencies in developing multi-level and multi-sectorial plans to implement interventions; and developing ways to embed interventions in existing networks throughout development from inception to implementation [69].

Conclusion

In conclusion, interventions based on behavioral theory have been shown to be effective in changing health behavior. However, there is still need for more research on interventions that systematically and precisely map intervention content with theoretical determinants, and the need for greater transparency in the reporting of intervention content and protocols. Arguments that such behavioral interventions do not work in the real world based on observations that pandemics of non-communicable disease continue to rise, and large scale interventions have not shifted population-level participation in health behavior, as my colleague contends, are specious and miss the point. The issue is not that interventions based on behavior theory do not work in changing behavior in ‘real world’ contexts, they do, rather, it is a lack of investment in, and inadequate upscaling and implementation of, these interventions that has failed to translate their efficacy into sustained, long-term change at the population level.

AGAINST: Mike Weed - interventions based on behavioral theory do not work in the real world

Over 50 years positive population behaviors or health outcomes for nutrition and physical activity have fallen or flatlined globally, and in individual countries. Data shows: rising global obesity since 1975 [71], and in individual countries including England [72], Chile [73], and Australia [71]; falling or flatlining fruit and vegetable consumption in USA since 1994 [74], and in Japan [75] and Brazil [76] since 1965; and rising physical inactivity globally since 2001 [77], in Spain since 1995 [78], in USA since 1997 [79], and in China since 1989 [80]. For health outcomes, European Environment Agency data show loss of healthy life years attributable to non-communicable diseases has grown by more than 20% since 1990 [81]. These data illustrate global trends, and their replication in individual countries.

Something isn’t working! Noting disjoint between a body of behavioral theory literature that appears to show promise at the individual level, and global and national data that shows no change in population behaviors and health outcomes for half a century, Hallal et al. [82] argue “after more than 60 years of scientific research… more of the same (in terms of research and practice) will not be enough” (p. 190). It appears behavioral theory has a case to answer, and some fundamental questions to face. But is the problem scale-up of behavioral theory in population level interventions and policies, is it intervention designs that act as the vehicle for behavioral theory, or is it simply that behavioral theory itself does not work in the real world?

Behavioral theory interventions are efficacious not effective

For decades fields such as exercise physiology, public health, epidemiology and the behavioral sciences have undertaken research showing that if behavioral theory is deployed “under scientifically controlled circumstances, behavior change is achievable for increasing physical activity” [69] (p. 1337). However, many “so-called effective physical activity interventions” (p. 1337) are small-scale, controlled efficacy trials that do not demonstrate effectiveness or ecological validity, and leave gaps in the chain of evidence between participants, theory, behavior and health outcomes [83]. An intervention is efficacious if it works in cohorts who receive it, whereas it is effective if it works in cohorts who have been offered it [84]. This is confused in the literature, and interventions based on behavioral theory claim effectiveness when available evidence demonstrates only their efficacy [69].

Many trials of interventions based on behavioral theory do not venture beyond controlled environments of phase I-III trials, which seek to establish, respectively, concept, efficacy and comparative efficacy. Thus, at best, evidence demonstrates that impact on those who receive the intervention exceeds impact on those who receive alternative interventions. But still, this shows only that an intervention is comparatively efficacious for those who receive it, not that it is effective, or comparatively effective, in cohorts that are offered it [84].

The problem is this: the features of design and implementation associated with good phase I-III trials to establish concept, efficacy, and comparative efficacy, have important limitations for informing practice and policy decisions [85], which require more generalizable information relating to outcomes of societal consequence, such as a sustained impact on health outcomes at population level. Such impact, or the potential for it, must relate to real world effectiveness “as evaluated in an observational, non-interventional trial in a naturalistic setting” [86]. To establish effectiveness, phase IV trials require a more diverse set of methods than those required to establish concept, efficacy and comparative efficacy in phase I-III trials, and must involve a diversity of settings, participants and deliverers [87]. However, in reviewing studies purporting to examine effectiveness of physical activity interventions in the real world (i.e., phase IV trials), Beedie, Mann and Jimenez [88] found that many still tried to adopt laboratory style methods and controls that would be impractical or uneconomic in real-world settings [89].

Some authors [69, 84, 90] have advocated the RE-AIM framework as a Phase IV tool to develop the effectiveness of interventions shown to be efficacious at phases I-III. But, with its focus on ensuring reach, adoption, implementation integrity, and maintenance of the features of the intervention over time, RE-AIM merely attempts to deliver effectiveness by maintaining the controlled environment of phase I-III trials in the real world, which as well as being impractical or uneconomic [89], is also likely to be futile.

Establishing effectiveness in phase IV trials is difficult, and requires longer timescales, and greater scale and resources than establishing concept, efficacy and comparative efficacy in phase I-III trials. As such, it is not surprising that, in an area where research funding is relatively sparse, and doctoral studies (which are time and resource limited) are often the bricks contributing to edifices of knowledge, genuine phase IV effectiveness trials are rare [89]. Nevertheless, there is a moral obligation to conduct them [87], otherwise advocacy for behavioral theory interventions based only on efficacy evidence risks wasting participants time (and hopes for their health) and taxpayers money on unproven interventions in unproven populations.

Behavioral theory interventions are recipients not stimuli of behavior change

Analyses of national participation data suggest interventions based on behavioral theory may be recipients of individual behavior change, rather than the stimulus for it. This is because populations’ behaviors are qualitatively different to individual behaviors, and incorporate individual behavioral volatility within their steady state. For example, in England two national surveys, Active People (n = 150,000+) and Taking Part (n = 15,000+) [91, 92], show population participation in sport and related physical activity has flatlined for 10 years, with no sustained change beyond +/− 2%. Furthermore, data synthesis across six surveys shows falling or flatlining participation for 25 years [83]. However, both cross-sectional retrospective report data and panel time-series data from the surveys also shows considerable individual behavioral volatility, with circa 20% of the population dropping out or doing less sport, 20% taking up or doing more sport, 20% maintaining participation, and 40% consistently doing no sport [83]. Consequently, within any 1 year circa 40% of the population change their sport participation behavior, but aggregate population level participation is unchanged. Thus, steady state population behaviors incorporate considerable individual behavioral change. This suggests behavioral theory interventions are reflecting and facilitating individual behavior changes that take place as part of the steady state behaviors of populations, with participants often presenting as already motivated to change [88, 93]. Sport England’s Get Active: Get Healthy [94] first-year pilots, for example, claimed to be the stimulus for more than 30,000 people becoming active, but the evaluation showed the majority of participants were “ready to change” when they joined. This suggests the interventions were the recipients rather than the stimulus for individual behavioral changes, which are to be expected as a normal part of steady state population behaviors.

Behavioral theory interventions are not linked to the causes of behavior

A health outcome stubbornly maintained in steady state population behaviors is widespread health inequality. It is known that poor health outcomes, particularly non-communicable diseases, correlate with social deprivation, low employment, poverty, poor housing, and other indices of multiple deprivation [95]. Behavioral theory provides neither the explanation nor, through interventions targeting individuals, the solution to such problems, which must focus on wider causal systems that underpin the social practice and economy of behaviors such as low physical activity and poor diet.

Undoubtedly, it is the focus on the individual rather than the population that undermines the real-world effectiveness of behavioral theory. The etiological model on which it is based – that poor health outcomes are caused by exposure to a substance, for example, sugar, and that health outcomes can be improved by modifying or moderating individual behaviors to remove or reduce exposure [96] – is fundamentally flawed. This is because solutions – interventions based on behavioral theory – have no relationship to causes – the factors that lead to behaviors in the first place. Furthermore, behavioral theory is assumed to be universal: that is, it is assumed the same behavioral theory can address any behavior, be that smoking, alcohol consumption, poor diet, or low physical activity – the transtheoretical model, which was developed for smoking cessation, is a case in point [97,98,99,100]. Cleary these behaviors are underpinned by different antecedents, so why would we assume they can all be addressed by the same theory? Furthermore, categories of behavior are not homogenous – the existence of health inequalities is, in itself, evidence that the factors that lead to behaviors in relation to, for example, diet, differ across the population, and so poor diet is an agglomeration of behaviors rather than a single behavior. Why would we expect that these multiple complex behaviors could all be addressed by the same theory?

Conclusion

I have argued that while interventions based on behavioral theory have been shown to be efficacious in the controlled environments of phase I-III trials, there is no evidence from genuine phase IV effectiveness trials to demonstrate they work in the real world. However, crucially, I argue that evidence from controlled trials of behavior change interventions simply capture individual behavioral volatility that is a normal part of steady state population behaviors. Furthermore, such interventions fail in shifting population behaviors because they focus on individuals rather than on the multiple complex factors that drive the distribution of behaviors in the population. As such, behavioral theory within such interventions is not an active ingredient, rather it is a dormant recipient of behavior change. Put simply, behavioral theory has no active influence on changing behaviors in the real world.

RESPONSE: Martin S. Hagger

I am grateful to my colleague for raising important points on the implementation of theory-based behavioral interventions and the need for more evidence for the effectiveness of behavioral interventions in ‘phase IV’ trials. These are good points that have been made many times elsewhere [64], including my opening statement. However, as an argument against the proposal, his statement is not fit-for-purpose. As I predicted, my colleague claims that interventions based on behavioral theory do not work in changing behavior in ‘real world’ contexts because there has been no year-on-year change in rates of non-communicable diseases and health-related behavior participation at the population level. He also suggests that behavior theory focuses solely on individual behavior, targets only the motivated, and fails to incorporate structural determinants of behavior. Here I illustrate how his arguments reflect a poor understanding of behavioral theory, and are not based on appropriate evidence, or, in some instances, any evidence at all.

My colleague’s argument that interventions based on behavioral theory do not work is flawed. He equates “behavioral interventions” with “theoretical basis”, and therefore claims theory-based interventions do not work because behavioral interventions have not been shown to work. As I argued in my opening statement, it is important to make a clear distinction between interventions based on behavioral theory, those merely ‘inspired’ by behavioral theory, and those that do not encompass theory at all [8, 56]. My colleague fails to make this distinction, and ignores evidence demonstrating the efficacy, and effectiveness, of interventions demonstrably based on behavioral theory in real world contexts [36, 41, 43, 101,102,103].

A further problem with his argument is to cite evidence of population-level non-change in rates of non-communicable disease and health-related behavior as evidence that behavioral theory does not work in the real world. It’s a poor argument without foundation. This is typified in his argument that the 25-year “falling or flatlining” of physical activity participation is somehow illustrative of a failure of behavioral interventions. This inference, which is speculative, is based on survey data on sport in which no intervention is identified. To make matters worse, this argument also infers that population-level changes in sport participation should reflect a desirable health-related outcome, a position he himself has argued against [83].

He also argues that considerable flux occurs in individuals’ behavior over time while a “steady state” is generally observed, suggesting that behavioral interventions capture this “volatility” rather than actual change. Yet, no evidence on theory-based behavioral interventions is offered to illustrate this point – the “Get Active: Get Healthy” campaign he cites, a sport-oriented intervention without basis in behavioral theory, provides no relevant data to verify this claim. Researchers designing trials of behavioral interventions are all too aware of the issue of time-dependent variability in health behaviors, and include appropriate covariates in their analyses to demonstrate intervention effectiveness is in spite of, not because of, population-level variation in behavior. However, better evidence than that cited by my colleague supports his contention that population level change in health behaviors has not been achieved [104]. Behavioral scientists’ should shoulder some of the blame for this failure by not advocating better implementation of effective interventions, but so too should all involved in the ‘chain of development’ of behavioral intervention from basic research to implementation.

My colleague also argues that: “Behavioral theory can provide neither the explanation…on the wider causal systems that underpin… behaviors such as low physical activity and poor diet”. This argument is incorrect, my colleague probably equates all behavioral theories as theories of individual behavior, which reflects a deficient knowledge of behavioral theory. Many behavioral theories incorporate socio-demographic, structural, and group-level variables as determinants of behavior, and propose how they interact with psychological determinants [21, 32, 39, 40]. Similarly, my colleague argues that: “Undoubtedly, it is the focus on the individual rather than the population that undermines the real-world effectiveness of behavioral theory.” I agree that a simple causal narrative, such as a sole focus on individual behavior change [96], will not be effective in reversing population-level incidence of non-communicable disease. However, this is not a failure of behavioral theory per se; many theories encompass individual, structural, and ecological determinants of behavior [105]. Rather, it points to a need to incorporate interventions based on behavioral theory into policy and practice through advances in implementation science [65,66,67].

Finally, my colleague suggests that theory-based behavioral interventions target only the motivated. This is not a new argument, intervention designers have been aware of this problem for years, and it is a problem that pervades mostinterventions, regardless of their theoretical basis. However, this is not the case for all interventions, and some of the most effective interventions work in changing behavior independent of motivation and in ‘real world’ contexts without the strict controls associated with laboratory research [36, 51, 102].

In conclusion, I commend my colleague for identifying the need for more effectiveness trials and translation efforts for theory-based behavioral interventions. However, his arguments against the effectiveness of behavioral theory in ‘real world’ contexts reflect an acute lack of understanding of behavioral theory, are based on incorrect inferences regarding behavioral theory, fall back on emotive language in an attempt to persuade, make no practical suggestions on the way forward for behavior change, and, as a consequence, should be summarily dismissed.

RESPONSE: Mike Weed

The arguments for behavioral theory barely warrant rebuttal. The volume of evidence presented for effectiveness [25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,50,51] demonstrates that those who receive behavioral theory interventions show changes in behavior compared to those who do not. This is evidence of efficacy, not effectiveness, a distinction not well understood in the literature. The appeal to implementation science is an attempt to extend controlled efficacy trial environments into implementation, focusing, for example, on maximising intervention fidelity [66] to achieve an illusion of effectiveness that is impractical, uneconomic and largely futile for achieving behavior change at scale. Finally, the litany of under-funded and unsustained interventions, which are presented as a result of poor communication by behavioral theorists and poor understanding by policy makers, suggests behavioral theorists are collaborating in their own victim narrative. Investment in implementing interventions has not been sustained because interventions have not been shown to be effective. But worst, because “implementation science” has sought to extend the controls of phase II and III efficacy and comparative efficacy trials into implementation, opportunities have been squandered to upgrade evidence by conducting genuine phase IV observational, non-interventional effectiveness trials in naturalistic settings.

If not behavioral theory, then what?

Behaviors change regularly and often. In England, circa 18 million adults change their sport participation each year [91, 92], yet population sport participation levels have remained stable for quarter of a century [83]. Shifting the curve of population behaviors requires an entirely different approach than changing individual behavior. Successes in the former include the use of seatbelts, and reductions in drink-driving and in smoking in public spaces, but they result from legislative mandating, not effective behavioral theory interventions.

Since the 1960s in England, tobacco advertising, and then tobacco sponsorship, was regulated, restricted and then banned, followed by increasing restrictions and then a ban on smoking in public spaces, with a ban in cars and rented social housing now also being considered. Latterly, warnings then graphic images of increasing severity and size have been required on tobacco products, which now cover the whole packaging. Legislation has regulated messages and mandated behaviors, including mandated engagement with efficacious fear appeal interventions to ensure intervention fidelity and deliver effectiveness that would not otherwise be possible. Now, 50 years on, society no longer supports the social practice of smoking, and not only is the tobacco industry not permitted to reinforce smoking as a desirable behavior, it is required to undermine it. The role of behavioral theory in this process has been minimal; success is attributable to understanding the meanings attached to smoking as a social practice [106], the ways in which it is reinforced [107], and to addressing social and economic causation [96] through incremental legislative mandating [108] that disrupts the social practice of smoking.

Is it time for a scientific revolution?

The academic practice of the development of behavioral theory shows the signs of paradigmatic science [109]. Theorists become self-reinforcing and self-referential devotees, advocates for theory rather than outcomes. Empirical deficiencies are attributed to imprecise specification or poor implementation, prompting calls for more meticulous use and more controlled implementation, or for tweaks at the margins of theory. Social ecological approaches, which co-opt social perspectives to support existing individualistic behavioral solutions, rather than to interrogate and understand social and economic causation, are an example of the latter. Kuhn [109] suggests these circumstances create the structure for scientific revolutions, in which empirical deficiencies can no longer be explained away at the margins or blamed on methods, and the old paradigm is displaced in favour of a new approach. I propose that new approach should be a social practice framework [96, 107, 109] that deploys legislative mandating as a tool to disrupt social practices, underpinned by understanding of social and economic causation. This should displace the current dominant individualistic behavioral paradigm that provides solutions that are not connected to causes. It’s time to burn down the house: the time for revolution is now!

Joint conclusion

Although the current debate has showcased our different perspectives, it has also highlighted points of agreement. We both agree that interventions based on behavioral theory are efficacious in changing health-related behaviors. We also agree that there are problems with current evidence for the effectiveness of behavioral interventions, but we disagree on the nature and extent of these problems and their implications for drawing conclusions about the ‘real world’ effectiveness of behavioral theory. Beyond this, we also disagree on the implications of the evidence base as it stands for advancing effective, long-term solutions to the increasing prevalence of non-communicable diseases.

Hagger

While evidence for real world effectiveness of interventions based on behavioral theory applied in real world contexts is limited, it is not absent. Good examples of theory-based interventions that have demonstrable real-world effectiveness in changing behavior exist (e.g., graphic warnings on tobacco products). Behavioral interventions offer a range of strategies that, if appropriately implemented, can and will make lasting changes in behavior at the population level. However, I recognize the need to develop the evidence base of effective large-scale behavioral interventions that can be embedded within existing networks, and are sensitive to the social and cultural norms of the target population. The interventions need to be sustainable through, for example, their incorporation into routine care or standard practice. Those developing interventions need to actively engage and lobby policymakers and governments to invest in interventions with demonstrated effectiveness and include them as core components of existing services. Behavioral interventions should be an integral part of a co-ordinated set of strategies that also includes policy change and legislation targeting change in specific behaviors at the population-level.

Weed

Fundamental change is required: a paradigm shift to focus on social practice rather than individual behavior. Evidence that behavioral theory interventions are genuinely effective among those offered them, rather than simply efficacious among those receiving them, is all but absent, and absence of evidence is evidence of absence. The effectiveness gap is one of engagement that cannot be bridged by persuasion, rather mandating is required, either through legislation, or through interventions with mandatory engagement, such as point of choice information. Nonetheless, there is a role for behavioral theory: firstly, in providing efficacious support for individuals wishing to change; secondly, as a minor dimension of a social practice approach, which places historic and contemporary social and economic forces that lead to the existence of social practices, and that sustain them, at the centre, rather than the contemporary behaviors of individuals. Social practices can be disrupted over time through the incremental interplay of legislative mandating, and social change that creates the conditions for legislation. However, the circumstances and pace of disruption are rooted in understanding social and economic causation, and how this underpins the distribution and acceptance of behaviors in a population, not in aggregative attempts to effect individual behavior change.

References

  1. Jemal A, Bray F, Center MM, Ferlay J, Ward E, Forman D. Global cancer statistics. CA-Cancer J Clin. 2011;61:69–90.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Loef M, Walach H. The combined effects of healthy lifestyle behaviors on all cause mortality: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Prev Med. 2012;55:163–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Hackshaw A, Morris JK, Boniface S, Tang J-L, Milenković D. Low cigarette consumption and risk of coronary heart disease and stroke: meta-analysis of 141 cohort studies in 55 study reports. BMJ. 2018;360:j5855.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  4. Wyatt SB, Winters KP, Dubbert PM. Overweight and obesity: prevalence, consequences, and causes of a growing public health problem. Am J Med Sci. 2006;166–174:166–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Finucane MM, Stevens GA, Cowan MJ, Danaei G, Lin JK, Paciorek CJ, Singh GM, Gutierrez HR, Lu Y, Bahalim AN, et al. National, regional, and global trends in body-mass index since 1980: systematic analysis of health examination surveys and epidemiological studies with 960 country-years and 9.1 million participants. Lancet. 2011;377:557–67.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  6. Michie S, Abraham C. Identifying techniques that promote health behaviour change: evidence based or evidence inspired? Psychol Health. 2004;19:29–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  7. Glanz K, Bishop DB. The role of behavioral science theory in development and implementation of public health interventions. Ann Rev Pub Health. 2010;31:399–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Michie S, Carey RN, Johnston M, Rothman AJ, de Bruin M, Kelly MP, Connell LE. From theory-inspired to theory-based interventions: a protocol for developing and testing a methodology for linking behaviour change techniques to theoretical mechanisms of action. Ann Behav Med. 2017;52:501–12.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Sheeran P, Klein WMP, Rothman AJ. Health behavior change: moving from observation to intervention. Ann Rev Psychol. 2017;68:573–600.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Johnson BT, Acabchuk RL. What are the keys to a longer, happier life? Answers from five decades of health psychology research. Soc Sci Med. 2018;196:218–26.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Klein WMP, Shepperd JA, Suls J, Rothman AJ, Croyle RT. Realizing the promise of social psychology in improving public health. Personal Soc Psychol Rev. 2015;19:77–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Rothman AJ, Klein WMP, Cameron LD. Advancing innovations in social/personality psychology and health: opportunities and challenges. Health Psychol. 2013;32:602–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Kelly MP, Barker M. Why is changing health-related behaviour so difficult? Pub Health. 2016;136:109–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Davis R, Campbell R, Hildon Z, Hobbs L, Michie S. Theories of behaviour and behaviour change across the social and behavioural sciences: a scoping review. Health Psychol Rev. 2015;9:323–44.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. King A. Theory's role in shaping behavioral health research for population health. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2015;12:146.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  16. Teixeira PJ, Carraca E, Markland DA, Silva M, Ryan RM. Exercise, physical activity, and self-determination theory: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:78.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  17. Ferguson E. Personality is of central concern to understand health: towards a theoretical model for health psychology. Health Psychol Rev. 2013;7:S32–70.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  18. Kelder SH, Hoelscher DM, Perry CL. How individuals, environments, and health behaviors interact. In: Glanz K, Rimer BK, Viswanath K, editors. Health behavior: Theory, research, and practice. 5th ed. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 2015. p. 159–82.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Puggina A, Aleksovska K, Buck C, Burns C, Cardon G, Carlin A, Chantal S, Ciarapica D, Condello G, Coppinger T, et al. Policy determinants of physical activity across the life course: a ‘DEDIPAC’ umbrella systematic literature review. Eur J Pub Health. 2017;28:105–18.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. Sallis JF, Owen N, Fisher EB. Ecological models of health behavior. In: Glanz K, Rimer BK, Viswanath K, editors. Health behavior and health education: Theory, research, and practice. 5th ed. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 2015. p. 43–64.

    Google Scholar 

  21. Hovell M, Wahlgren D, Adams M. The logical and empirical basis for the behavioral ecological model. In: Emerging theories in health promotion practice and research. 2nd ed. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass; 2009. p. 415–49.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Abraham C. Mapping modifiable mechanisms in health promotion research: a commentary on Sniehotta, Presseau, and Araújo-Soares. Health Psychol Rev. 2015;9:160–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Massey SH, Decety J, Wisner KL, Wakschlag LS. Specification of change mechanisms in pregnant smokers for malleable target identification: a novel approach to a tenacious public health problem. Front Public Health. 2017;5:12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Kok G, Gottlieb NH, Peters G-JY, Mullen PD, Parcel GS, Ruiter RAC, Fernández ME, Markham C, Bartholomew LK. A taxonomy of behavior change methods: An intervention mapping approach. Health Psychol Rev. 2016;10:297–312.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Knittle K, Nurmi J, Crutzen R, Hankonen N, Beattie M, Dombrowski SU. How can interventions increase motivation for physical activity? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. 2018;12:211–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Silva MN, Vieira PN, Coutinho SR, Minderico CS, Matos MG, Sardinha LB, Teixeira PJ. Using self-determination theory to promote physical activity and weight control: a randomized controlled trial in women. J Behav Med. 2010;33:110–22.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Steinmetz H, Knappstein M, Ajzen I, Schmidt P, Kabst R. How effective are behavior change interventions based on the theory of planned behavior? A three-level meta-analysis. Z Psychol-J Psychol. 2016;224:216–33.

    Google Scholar 

  28. Sheeran P, Maki A, Montanaro E, Avishai-Yitshak A, Bryan A, Klein WMP, Miles E, Rothman AJ. The impact of changing attitudes, norms, and self-efficacy on health-related intentions and behavior: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 2016;35:1178–88.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Tessier D, Sarrazin P, Nicaise V, Dupont JP. The effects of persuasive communication and planning on intentions to be more physically active and on physical activity behaviour among low-active adolescents. Psychol Health. 2015;30:583–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Williams GC, McGregor HA, Sharp D, Kouides RW, Levesque CS, Ryan RM, Deci EL. A self-determination multiple risk intervention trial to improve smokers’ health. J Gen Intern Med. 2006;21:1288–94.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Albarracín D, Gillette JC, Earl AN, Glasman LR, Durantini MR, Ho MH. A test of major assumptions about behavior change: a comprehensive look at the effects of passive and active HIV-prevention interventions since the beginning of the epidemic. Psychol Bull. 2005;131:856–97.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Heath GW, Parra DC, Sarmiento OL, Andersen LB, Owen N, Goenka S, Montes F, Brownson RC. Evidence-based intervention in physical activity: lessons from around the world. Lancet. 2012;380:272–81.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  33. May S, West R. Do social support interventions (“buddy systems”) aid smoking cessation? A review. Tob Con. 2000;9:415–22.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Wakefield MA, Loken B, Hornik RC. Use of mass media campaigns to change health behaviour. Lancet. 2010;376:1261–71.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Brewer NT, Hall MG, Noar SM. Pictorial cigarette pack warnings increase quitting: a comment on Kok et al. Health Psychol Rev. 2018;12:129–32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  36. Brewer NT, Hall MG, Noar SM, Parada H, Stein-Seroussi A, Bach LE, Hanley S, Ribisl KM. Effect of pictorial cigarette pack warnings on changes in smoking behavior: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Int Med. 2016;176:905–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Borland R. Misinterpreting theory and ignoring evidence: fear appeals can actually work: a comment on Kok et al. (2018). Health Psychol Rev. 2018;12:126–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Durkin S, Brennan E, Wakefield MA. Mass media campaigns to promote smoking cessation among adults: An integrative review. Tob Con. 2012;21:127–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Doran K, Resnick B, Kim N, Lynn D, McCormick T. Applying the social ecological model and theory of self-efficacy in the worksite heart health improvement project-PLUS. Res Theor Nurs Prac. 2017;31:8–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Aittasalo M, Tiilikainen J, Tokola K, Seimelä T, Sarjala S-M, Metsäpuro P, Hynynen A, Suni J, Sievänen H, Vähä-Ypyä H, et al. Socio-ecological intervention to promote active commuting to work: protocol and baseline findings of a cluster randomized controlled trial in Finland. Int J Environ Res Pub Health. 2017;14:1257.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  41. Allais O, Bazoche P, Teyssier S. Getting more people on the stairs: the impact of point-of-decision prompts. Soc Sci Med. 2017;192:18–27.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Kremers SPJ, Eves FF, Andersen RE. Environmental changes to promote physical activity and healthy dietary behavior. J Environ Pub Health. 2012;2012:4.

    Google Scholar 

  43. Lewis AL, Eves FF. Prompts to increase stair climbing in stations: the effect of message complexity. J Phys Act Health. 2012;9:954–61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Waterlander WE, Steenhuis IHM, Boer MR, Schuit AJ, Seidell JC. The effects of a 25% discount on fruits and vegetables: results of a randomized trial in a three-dimensional web-based supermarket. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2012;9:11.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Marteau TM, Ogilvie D, Roland M, Suhrcke M, Kelly MP. Judging nudging: can nudging improve population health? BMJ. 2011;342:d228.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Venema TAG, Kroese FM, De Ridder DTD. I’m still standing: a longitudinal study on the effect of a default nudge. Psychol Health. 2017;33:669–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Michie S, Abraham C, Whittington C, McAteer J, Gupta S. Effective techniques in healthy eating and physical activity interventions: a meta-regression. Health Psychol. 2009;28:690–701.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Olander E, Fletcher H, Williams S, Atkinson L, Turner A, French D. What are the most effective techniques in changing obese individuals’ physical activity self-efficacy and behaviour: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2013;10:29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Stead M, Craigie A, Macleod M, McKell J, Caswell S, Steele R, Anderson A. Why are some people more successful at lifestyle change than others? Factors associated with successful weight loss in the BeWEL randomised controlled trial of adults at risk of colorectal cancer. Int J Behav Med Phys Act. 2015;12:87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Carfora V, Caso D, Conner MT. Randomized controlled trial of a messaging intervention to increase fruit and vegetable intake in adolescents: Affective versus instrumental messages. Br J Health Psychol. 2016;21:937–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Hollands GJ, Shemilt I, Marteau TM, Jebb SA, Lewis HB, Wei Y, Higgins JPT, Ogilvie D. Portion, package or tableware size for changing selection and consumption of food, alcohol and tobacco. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015;9:CD011045.

    Google Scholar 

  52. Prestwich A, Sniehotta FF, Whittington C, Dombrowski SU, Rogers L, Michie S. Does theory influence the effectiveness of health behavior interventions? Meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 2014;33:465–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Conn VS, Hafdahl AR, Cooper PS, Brown LM, Lusk SL. Meta-analysis of workplace physical activity interventions. Am J Prev Med. 2009;37:330–9.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. McDermott MS, Oliver M, Iverson D, Sharma R. Effective techniques for changing physical activity and healthy eating intentions and behaviour: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Br J Health Psychol. 2016;21:827–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Goodwin L, Ostuzzi G, Khan N, Hotopf MH, Moss-Morris R. Can we identify the active ingredients of behaviour change interventions for coronary heart disease patients? A systematic review and meta-analysis. PLoS ONE. 2016;11:e0153271.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  56. Michie S, Prestwich A. Are interventions theory-based? Development of a theory coding scheme. Health Psychol. 2010;29:1–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Michie S, Carey R, Johnston M, Rothman AJ, Kelly M, Davidson K, de Bruin M. From theory-inspired to theory-based interventions: linking behaviour change techniques to their mechanisms of action. Eur Health Psychol. 2016;18:395.

    Google Scholar 

  58. Michie S, Abraham C. Advancing the science of behaviour change: a plea for scientific reporting. Addiction. 2008;103:1409–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Marteau TM, Dieppe P, Foy R, Kinmonth A-L, Schneiderman N. Behavioural medicine: changing our behaviour. BMJ. 2006;332:437–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  60. Russell LM, Rubin GL, Leeder SR. Preventive health reform: what does it mean for public health? Med J Austr. 2008;188:715–9.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Abu-Omar K, Rütten A, Burlacu I, Schätzlein V, Messing S, Suhrcke M. The cost-effectiveness of physical activity interventions: a systematic review of reviews. Prev Med Rep. 2017;8:72–8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  62. Marteau TM. Changing minds about changing behaviour. Lancet. 2018;391:116–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Rothman AJ. Capitalizing on opportunities to refine health behavior theories. Health Educ Behav. 2009;36:150S–5S.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  64. Glasgow RE, Klesges LM, Dzewaltowski DA, Bull SS, Estabrooks P. The future of health behavior change research: what is needed to improve translation of research into health promotion practice? Ann Behav Med. 2004;27:3–12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Michie S, West R. Behaviour change theory and evidence: a presentation to government. Health Psychol Rev. 2013;7:1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Bauer MS, Damschroder L, Hagedorn H, Smith J, Kilbourne AM. An introduction to implementation science for the non-specialist. BMC Psychol. 2015;3:32.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  67. Moullin J, Sabater-Hernández D, Benrimoj S. Development of a theoretically based implementation protocol. BMC Health Serv Res. 2014;14:P83.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Rosenberg D, Kerr J, Sallis JF, Patrick K, Moore DJ, King A. Feasibility and outcomes of a multilevel place-based walking intervention for seniors: a pilot study. Health Place. 2009;15:173–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Reis RS, Salvo D, Ogilvie D, Lambert EV, Goenka S, Brownson RC. Scaling up physical activity interventions worldwide: stepping up to larger and smarter approaches to get people moving. Lancet. 2016;388:1337–48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  70. Diepeveen S, Ling T, Suhrcke M, Roland M, Marteau TM. Public acceptability of government intervention to change health-related behaviours: a systematic review and narrative synthesis. BMC Pub Health. 2013;13:756.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. World Obesity Federation. Trends in globl obesity. London: World Obesity Federation; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  72. Health Survey for England. Health survey for England, 2016. London: Office of National Statistics; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  73. World Obesity Federation. Trends in prevalence of obesity in adults from selected emerging countries. London: World Obesity Federation; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  74. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. 2018 state indicator report on fruits and vegtables, vol. 2018. Atlanta: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Ministry of Agriculture Forestry and Fisheries. Report of survey on markets of vegetables and fruits. Tokyo: MAFF; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Food and Agriculture Association of the United Nations. Country report: Brazil. Rome: FAO; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  77. Guthold R, Stevens GA, Riley LM, Bull FC. Worldwide trends in insufficient physical activity from 2001 to 2016: A pooled analysis of 358 population-based surveys with 19 million participants. Lancet Glob Health. 2018;6:e1077–86.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Meseguer CM, Galán I, Herruzo R, Rodríguez-Artalejo F. Tendencias de actividad física en tiempo libre y en el trabajo en la Comunidad de Madrid, 1995-2008. Rev Esp Cardiol. 2011;64:21–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  79. Physical Activity Guidelines Advisory Committee. Physical activity guidelines advisory committee report. Washington, DC: DHHS; 2008.

    Google Scholar 

  80. Zhang B, Zhai FY, Du SF, Popkin BM. The China health and nutrition survey, 1989–2011. Obes Rev. 2014;15:2–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  81. European Environment Agency. The shift in global disease burden, and share of non-communicable diseases by world regions. Copenhagen: EEA; 2017.

    Google Scholar 

  82. Hallal PC, Bauman AE, Heath GW, Kohl HW 3rd, Lee IM, Pratt M. Physical activity: more of the same is not enough. Lancet. 2012;380:190–1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  83. Weed ME. Should we privilege sport for health? The comparative effectiveness of UK government investment in sport as a public health intervention. Int J Sport Policy Polit. 2016;8:559–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  84. Courneya KS. Efficacy, effectiveness, and behavior change trials in exercise research. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010;7:81.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  85. Wells KB. Treatment research at the crossroads: the scientific interface of clinical trials and effectiveness research. Am J Psychiat. 1999;156:5–10.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Suvarna V. Phase IV of drug development. Persp Clin Res. 2010;1:57–60.

    Google Scholar 

  87. Hill TP. Conducting phase IV clinical studies: A moral imperative? ecancer. 2012;6:276.

    Google Scholar 

  88. Beedie C, Mann S, Jimenez A. Community fitness center-based physical activity interventions: a brief review. Curr Sport Med Rep. 2014;13:267–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Beedie C, Mann S, Jimenez A, Kennedy L, Lane AM, Domone S, Wilson S, Whyte G. Death by effectiveness: exercise as medicine caught in the efficacy trap! Br J Sports Med. 2016;50:323–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  90. Jilcott S, Ammerman AS, Sommers J, Glasgow RE. Applying the RE-AIM framework to assess the public health impact of policy change. Ann Behav Med. 2007;34:105–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  91. DCMS. Taking part survey. London: Department for Digital, Culture, Media and Sport; 2018.

    Google Scholar 

  92. England S. Active people survey. London: Sport England; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  93. Dunn AL, Marcus BH, Kampert JB, Garcia ME, Kohl HW, Blair SN. Reduction in cardiovascular disease Rrisk factors: 6-month results from ProjectActive. Prev Med. 1997;26:883–92.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  94. England S. Tackling inactivity. Sport England: London; 2016.

    Google Scholar 

  95. Marmot M. Social determinants of health inequalities. Lancet. 2005;365:1099–104.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  96. Kelly MP, Russo F. Causal narratives in public health: the difference between mechanisms of aetiology and mechanisms of prevention in non-communicable diseases. Sociol Health Ill. 2018;40:82–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  97. Fava JL, Velicer WF, Prochaska JO. Applying the transtheoretical model to a representative sample of smokers. Addict Behav. 1995;20:189–203.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  98. Carbonari JP, DiClemente CC. Using transtheoretical model profiles to differentiate levels of alcohol abstinence success. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2000;68:810–7.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  99. Finckenor M, Byrd-Bredbenner C. Nutrition intervention group program based on preaction-stage oriented change processes of the transtheoretical model promotes long-term reduction in dietary fat intake. J Acad Nutr Diet. 2000;100:335–42.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  100. Marcus BH, Simkin LR. The trantheoretical model: applications to exercise behaviour. J Am Coll Sport Med. 1994;26:1400–4.

    CAS  Google Scholar 

  101. Milkman KL, Beshears J, Choi JJ, Laibson D, Madrian BC. Using implementation intentions prompts to enhance influenza vaccination rates. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2011;108:10415–20.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  102. Benartzi S, Beshears J, Milkman KL, Sunstein CR, Thaler RH, Shankar M, Tucker-Ray W, Congdon WJ, Galing S. Should governments invest more in nudging? Psychol Sci. 2017;28:1041–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  103. Ali MK, Echouffo-Tcheugui JB, Williamson DF. How effective were lifestyle interventions in real-world settings that were modeled on the diabetes prevention program? Health Aff. 2012;31:67–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. An R, Xiang X, Yang Y, Yan H. Mapping the prevalence of physical inactivity in U.S. States, 1984–2015. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0168175.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  105. Stokols D. Establishing and maintaining healthy environments: Toward a social ecology of health promotion. Am Psychol. 1992;47:6–22.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  106. Cohn S. From health behaviours to health practices: An introduction. Sociol Health Ill. 2014;36:157–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  107. Blue S, Shove E, Carmona C, Kelly MP. Theories of practice and public health: understanding (un) healthy practices. Crit Pub Health. 2016;26:36–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  108. Bauld L. The impact of smokefree legislation in England: evidence review. London: Department of Health; 2011.

    Google Scholar 

  109. Kuhn T. The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicaco Press; 1962.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

None.

Funding

Martin Hagger’s contribution was supported by a Finland Distinguished Professor (FiDiPro) award (Dnro 1801/31/2105) from Business Finland.

Availability of data and materials

No data is available for this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

MSH and MW conceived the idea and drafted the manuscript. Both authors contributed equally to this article. Both authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Martin S. Hagger or Mike Weed.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

Ethical approval and consent is not applicable to this type of manuscript.

Consent for publication

MSH and MW consent to the content of the manuscript to be published as presented.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Hagger, M.S., Weed, M. DEBATE: Do interventions based on behavioral theory work in the real world?. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 16, 36 (2019). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12966-019-0795-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12966-019-0795-4

Keywords