Skip to main content

Predictors of screen viewing time in young Singaporean children: the GUSTO cohort

Abstract

Background

Higher screen viewing time (SVT) in childhood has been associated with adverse health outcomes, but the predictors of SVT in early childhood are poorly understood. We examined the sociodemographic and behavioral predictors of total and device-specific SVT in a Singaporean cohort.

Methods

At ages 2 and 3 years, SVT of 910 children was reported by their parents. Interviewer-administered questionnaires assessed SVT on weekdays and weekends for television, computer, and hand-held devices. Multivariable linear mixed-effect models were used to examine the associations of total and device-specific SVT at ages 2 and 3 with predictors, including children’s sex, ethnicity, birth order, family income, and parental age, education, BMI, and television viewing time.

Results

At age 2, children’s total SVT averaged 2.4 ± 2.2 (mean ± SD) hours/day, including 1.6 ± 1.6 and 0.7 ± 1.0 h/day for television and hand-held devices, respectively. At age 3, hand-held device SVT was 0.3 (95% CI: 0.2, 0.4) hours/day higher, while no increases were observed for other devices. SVT tracked moderately from 2 to 3 years (r = 0.49, p < 0.0001). Compared to Chinese children, Malay and Indian children spent 1.04 (0.66, 1.41) and 0.54 (0.15, 0.94) more hours/day watching screens, respectively. Other predictors of longer SVT were younger maternal age, lower maternal education, and longer parental television time.

Conclusions

In our cohort, the main predictors of longer children’s SVT were Malay and Indian ethnicity, younger maternal age, lower education and longer parental television viewing time. Our study may help target populations for future interventions in Asia, but also in other technology-centered societies.

Trial registration

This ongoing study was first registered on July 1, 2010 on NCT01174875 as. Retrospectively registered.

Background

In the 1960’s, television became widely introduced into homes of high-income countries and rapidly occupied a substantial fraction of individuals’ leisure time. In 2013, watching television reached an average of 3.5 h daily in US adults, i.e., about 10 years total accumulated over the life course [1]. Since the 1980’s, office-based workers have spent increasing part of their work day facing computer monitors, and more recently, playing video games has become a popular home activity for many Western adults and children [2,3,4]. Screens have now evolved to pocket, mobile, and personal devices, such as tablets and smartphones, which broaden the opportunities to use such devices in different locations and times. Importantly, hand-held devices have become increasingly accessible to young children and to individuals from lower socioeconomic strata and those from low- and middle-income countries.

Lack of physical activity is now recognized as a leading cause of preventable morbidity and mortality globally [5,6,7]. Less evidence is available, however, regarding high levels of sedentary behavior, which differs from inactivity [5, 8]. In adults, a sedentary lifestyle is an independent risk factor for cardiovascular and metabolic diseases [9]. In childhood, sedentary lifestyle has been independently associated with obesity, higher blood pressure and poorer mental health [9]. This level of evidence remains fragile, however, owing to cross-sectional designs, the complexity of accurately measuring sedentary behavior in young children, and the potential for residual confounding. Screen use is a waking activity involving low energy expenditure and is therefore considered a form of sedentary behavior [8, 10]. Screen viewing time (SVT) in preschool children is associated with eating disorders, reduced sleep duration, development delays, attention deficit and myopia [11,12,13]. As early as age 2, SVT has been associated with childhood obesity [14]. Based on this evidence, the American Academy of Pediatrics (AAP) now recommends that children below 18 months avoid any digital media use but suggests the gradual introduction of family-shared, high-quality content between 18 months and 2 years, while limiting screen time to a maximum of 1 h/day between 2 and 5 years of age [15].

Populations living in high-income Asian countries have become among the top users of all kinds of screen devices. According to the Google Consumer Barometer survey, in 2015 only 6% of persons ≥16 years living in South Korea or Singapore owned no devices (vs 13% in the U.S.) [16]. This survey also reported that 91% of Singaporeans owned a smartphone, 41% a tablet, and 60% at least 3 screen devices. The above statistics on prevalence of SVT have remained fairly stable in Singapore over recent years but are rising rapidly in Western countries [16]. In some ways, observations from “high-tech” Asian countries may presage a similar situation in Western populations. A multiscreen environment may encourage screen use in early childhood. To date, predictors of screen viewing behavior in Asian children are not well known; they may differ from those reported in other populations [17, 18]. As an illustration, a recent Singaporean cross-sectional study focusing on device-specific screen use in children less than 2 years of age reported a median number of 2 televisions, 2 computers, and 4 hand-held devices in their homes, while half of their parents had a total SVT ≥8 h daily [19]. Parents of less than half the sample of children aged <2 years reported no screen viewing, while 16% reported ≥2 h daily. Ethnicity, parental screen viewing and rule setting practices were identified as independent predictors of total SVT [19]. Longitudinal data are needed to establish the temporal precedence of potential predictors of SVT and changes in SVT over time. We thus aimed to describe total and device-specific SVT and assess its predictors, both cross-sectionally and longitudinally, among children aged 2 and 3 years enrolled in a tri-ethnic Singaporean cohort.

Methods

Study design and population

Between June 2009 and September 2010, the Growing Up in Singapore Towards healthy Outcomes (GUSTO) study recruited pregnant women of Chinese, Malay and Indian ethnicities who visited two public maternity units (KK Hospital and National University Hospital) in Singapore for their first ultrasound scan before the 15th week of pregnancy. The main exclusion criteria were non-homogeneous ethnic background (up to the four grandparents of the offspring), intention not to deliver in the study centers or not to remain in Singapore for the following 5 years. From the 1247 pregnant women recruited, 1171 singleton newborns were included and followed regularly. The recruitment and follow-up protocol has been detailed previously [20]. All participants signed written informed consent at enrolment. The study received ethical approval from the National Healthcare Group Domain Specific Review Board and the SingHealth Centralised Institutional Review Board.

Data collection

Sociodemographic and health information was obtained at enrolment as part of an interviewer-administered questionnaire: ethnicity (Chinese/Malay/Indian), maternal and paternal age, maternal highest education level (secondary or less/post-secondary/university), marital status (single/married), maternal place of birth (Singapore/other), accommodation type (public/private housing), monthly household income (<2000/2000-3999/4000-5999/≥6000 Singapore dollars), and maternal pre-pregnancy weight (kg). During the clinic visit at 26-28 weeks’ gestation, maternal height (cm) was measured, and pre-pregnancy and pregnancy behavioral information was obtained through an interviewer-administered questionnaire: tobacco (yes/no) and alcohol consumption (yes/no), television viewing time (6 categories from <1 h to >5 h daily) and physical activity (frequency and duration of light-moderate, moderate and vigorous intensity activities). Information on the offspring’s date of birth, sex and birth order was extracted from medical records. Paternal data were obtained from the fathers when they attended (80% of them did) postnatal visits with their child at 24 or 36 months: highest education level and television viewing time (same categories as for mothers) via interviewer-administered questionnaires, while weight (kg) and height (cm) were both measured. Time point and measurement method of the collected data are summarized in Additional file 1: Table S1.

Maternal and paternal age were categorized into 4 groups: <25, 25-29, 30-34 and ≥35 years for mothers and <30, 30-34, 35-39, and ≥40 years for fathers. Maternal pre-pregnancy and paternal BMI was derived from weight divided by height squared (m2), then categorized according to WHO criteria (<18.5, 18.5-24.9, 25.0-29.9, and ≥30.0 kg/m2). Maternal and paternal television viewing times were categorized as <1 h, 1-2 h, 2-3 h or ≥3 h/day. Maternal physical activity before pregnancy was categorized as insufficiently, sufficiently or highly active, as detailed previously [21].

Screen viewing time (SVT)

Screen use data were assessed at the 2- and 3-year clinic visits as part of a questionnaire on quantitating children’s time spent in indoor and outdoor activities. Trained interviewers asked the parents how much time, in 5-min increments, their child spent on average using screens on weekdays and weekend days. Three types of devices (with examples enunciated to the participants) were considered: 1) television and television games (e.g., PlayStation®, Wii™, Xbox™), further referred to as ‘television,’ 2) computers, and 3) hand-held video games and hand phones (e.g., Game Boy®, hand-phone games), including tablets, hereinafter referred to as hand-held devices. Although the item ‘computer’ was enunciated without example, the interviewers were trained to clarify, if necessary, that it includes both desktop and laptop computers. For each type, weekday and weekend day times were averaged to obtain device-specific SVT in hours/day ([weekday × 5 + weekend day × 2]/7). Total SVT was calculated as the sum of the times for the three types of device.

Statistical analyses

Total and device-specific SVT at 2 and 3 years, and the changes between those ages, are described as mean ± standard deviation (SD) and median and interquartile range (IQR), while categorical variables are summarized using frequencies and percentages. Spearman’s correlation was used to assess the degree of SVT tracking between 2 and 3 years.

Potential predictors of SVT at 2 and 3 years were examined in several steps. First, parental and child predictors were screened by linear regression to identify those associated with SVT in unadjusted models. Second, multivariable linear regression models were built using a forward selection approach and p ≤ 0.25 as an entering criterion. To maximize statistical power, multivariable analyses were restricted to the sample with complete data for both the offspring and the mother (n = 910), but not necessarily with complete paternal data (since they were missing for n = 270). Multivariable models with the paternal variables were built afterwards on the sample with complete data for the three family members (n = 640). We also carried out a sensitivity analysis using multiple imputation for missing paternal data. Third, repeated measures linear regression model with an unstructured variance-covariance matrix was used to account for non-independence among the repeated outcomes for each individual assessed at both time points. This also permitted us to use a single model for children whose SVT was estimated at one time point only (see Fig. 1). Interactions between the predictors and time points were tested to evaluate whether predictors were associated differentially with SVT at 2 and 3 years. Significant interaction denotes an association between the predictor and the change in SVT from 2 to 3 years [22], but has the additional advantage of including all children assessed at either age; p-interaction was considered significant when <0.10, since tests of interaction tend to be statistically under-powered. Interactions between child sex and ethnicity, and child sex and parental television time were also examined. Finally, we used multivariable logistic regression to examine whether the predictors of total SVT also predicted the odds of ≥2 and ≥4 h/day. All statistical analyses were carried out with SAS 9.4 (SAS Institute Inc., Cary, NC, USA) during the second half of 2016.

Fig. 1
figure 1

Flow diagram of the GUSTO study participants followed up to 2 and 3 years

Results

Overall, 910 parents reported their child’s SVT, including 812 reporting SVT at both ages (n = 812) and those who reported at 2 years only (n = 49) or 3 years only (n = 49) (Fig. 1). At age 2 years, the children spent an average of 2.4 ± 2.2 h of total SVT, mainly contributed by 1.6 ± 1.7 h of television and 0.7 ± 1.0 of hand-held devices (Table 1). Fewer children engaged in any computer use: 21 and 18% at 2 and 3 years, respectively. Thus, computer use was not examined further separately. At age 3 years, total SVT increased on average by 0.33 ± 2.42 h/day, mainly owing to an increase in hand-held device viewing time. SVTs at 2 and 3 years were moderately correlated (r = 0.45 for television, r = 0.42 hand-held devices, r = 0.49 for total SVT, all p < 0.0001, not shown).

Table 1 Daily total and device-specific SVT in 2- and 3-year-old children from the GUSTO cohort study

The study sample at 2 and 3 years of age is described in Table 2. Predictors of child SVT that passed the multivariable model selection criteria were ethnicity, household income, parental age and education, and higher maternal television viewing time (all p < 0.001). Marital status, maternal place of birth, accommodation type, maternal tobacco and alcohol consumption, and parental BMI did not reach the model selection criteria (p > 0.25, not shown) and were therefore excluded from the subsequent multivariable models. Although not associated with SVT, child sex and exact age were forced into the models.

Table 2 Sample description in 2- and 3-year-old children

Results of linear mixed-effect regression models adjusted for maternal data (model 1) and both parents’ data (model 2) are shown in Table 3. Compared to Chinese children, Malay and Indian children spent 0.94 (0.64, 1.25) and 0.60 (0.28, 0.92) more hours/day, respectively, in total SVT. Other sociodemographic variables independently associated with total or device-specific SVT were birth order, low household income, maternal education less than university level and paternal age < 35 years. Longer maternal and paternal television viewing times were associated with longer total SVT.

Table 3 Adjusted associations between predictors and total and device-specific SVT (h/day) in 2- and 3-year-old children

Interactions were observed between maternal age and child age (time point) and both television (p = 0.07) and hand-held device (p = 0.05) viewing time as outcomes (Additional file 2: Figure S1). Television viewing time at 2 years was longer in children of mothers aged <30 years than those of mothers ≥30 years, whereas no differences were observed at 3 years. The interaction pattern was opposite for hand-held device viewing time; no differences by maternal age were observed at age 2 years, whereas viewing time at age 3 years was longest in children of mothers <25 years. No other factors were significantly associated with the change in SVT between ages 2 and 3 years.

No interaction was found between child sex and ethnicity, nor between child sex and parental television viewing time. In logistic regression models, the predictors of total SVT ≥2 and ≥4 h/day were similar to those observed in the linear mixed-effect regressions (not shown). Finally, sensitivity analyses using multiple imputation for missing paternal viewing time showed comparable results (not shown).

Discussion

In our tri-ethnic Singaporean mother-offspring cohort, we found a substantial amount of reported total SVT in early childhood, mainly attributable to television and hand-held devices, whereas computers were used by few children. An increase in total SVT was seen from age 2 to 3 years, largely owing to an increase in hand-held device viewing time. SVT tracked moderately from age 2 to 3 years for all types of screen devices. We identified several predictors of SVT; Malay and Indian ethnicity, maternal education less than university level, and higher parental television viewing time were significantly and independently associated with longer total SVT. Predictors of device-specific viewing time differed somewhat. Low household income and high maternal daily television viewing time were associated with longer television viewing time, whereas being a first-born was associated with longer hand-held device viewing time. Finally, younger maternal age was a significant predictor of an increase in SVT between 2 and 3 years.

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study to use a longitudinal design to assess SVT and its predictors in Asians children. The GUSTO cohort is based in Singapore, a high-income country where screen use is among the highest globally [16]. At 2 years of age, about 75% of our study children exceeded the recent AAP recommendations for 2 and 3 year old children [15]. Similarly to previous reports in older children, we found that SVT tracks moderately between 2 and 3 years [23]. An increase in SVT between 2 and 3 years was mostly due to an increase in hand-held device SVT, consistent with findings from a cross-sectional study among Singaporean children aged 2 years and below. In that study, Goh et al. also reported that greater SVT at older ages was mainly attributable to use of hand-held devices [19]. Our study also corroborates their finding that young Malay and Indian children engage in longer SVT than Chinese children, even after controlling for other sociodemographic variables. Although the relationship between ethnicity and childhood SVT may be country- and context-specific, the available literature suggests that children from ethnic majorities spend less time on screen devices [18, 24,25,26].

Consistent with other studies on children of the same age, we did not observe a sex difference in SVT [14, 19, 24, 27,28,29]. However, it has been reported that boys spend more time on screen devices after age 4-5 years, suggesting that a difference by sex may emerge with age [30,31,32]. Among the studies that have examined birth order or number of siblings, most were conducted before 2010, when tablets and smartphones were not widely available [24, 28, 33,34,35,36]. Other sociodemographic factors associated with longer total or device-specific SVT in our study were low household income, maternal education less than university level, and paternal age less than 35 years. Paternal education was, however, not associated with SVT. Other studies have found similar associations, although the findings remain mixed and likely population-specific [18, 37]. Interestingly, we found that maternal age was associated with change in total and device-specific SVT between 2 and 3 years. Children of mothers younger than 25 years engaged in longer television watching at 2 years than did their counterparts, but from 2 to 3 years, they had a larger increase in hand-held device use. This finding may reflect changing parenting patterns of the so-called Generation Y, who grew up with television and now have switched to more recent digital technologies like tablets and smartphones.

Several parental behavioral predictors were also observed. Maternal television viewing time ≥ 3 h/day was among the strongest predictors of children’s total SVT and television viewing time, but not of hand-held device viewing time. Paternal television viewing time ≥ 2 h/day was associated, although less strongly, with total and device-specific SVT. Previous studies have reported that the presence of frequent screen users in the household is associated with children’s screen behavior [18, 36, 37]. Altogether, the evidence suggests that children’s screen use behavior is strongly influenced by parental behavior. Targeting parental behavior in early childhood may be a potentially effective avenue for interventions aimed at reducing children’s SVT.

Strengths of our study include its longitudinal design, wide range of sociodemographic and behavioral predictors measured a priori and assessment of SVT at two ages, a clear advantage over cross-sectional studies. We also assessed exposure to different types of screen devices, including hand-held devices, which has been uncommon in previous studies. We treated SVT as a continuous variable, thus providing greater statistical power vis-à-vis categorized variables. Study limitations include reliance on SVT reported by parents, who may overlook the child’s screen use when the parents are not at home or during daycare. Whilst the questionnaire we administered has been used in other studies [38], its validity and reliability are unknown. Moreover, it includes no information about the context of screen use (number of screens, parental knowledge on the recommendations, and rule-setting practices), nor on the viewing content. Contextual information is of importance when developing and testing interventions, as highlighted by the recent AAP guidelines that now included media content as an additional target. However, assessing both objective and qualitative SVT measures is challenging and costly in large epidemiological studies. We assessed maternal television viewing time during pregnancy, but it may have changed after birth. Finally, the GUSTO cohort is not representative of the entire Singaporean population. Malay and Indian families were overrepresented purposely at inclusion. GUSTO mothers were also less likely to hold a university degree than the women from the general population of the same age range [39]. The SVT figures observed in our study may therefore differ somewhat from those at the national level.

Conclusions

In conclusion, we observed a substantial total SVT in Singaporean children at ages 2 and 3 years. Significant predictors of total SVT included ethnicity, maternal education and parental television viewing, suggesting potential targets for health promotion activities. We observed stable television viewing time between 2 and 3 years, whereas hand-held device viewing time increased over time, particularly in children of young mothers. Intervention studies targeting parental screen behavior in early childhood may be warranted: for example, by testing the impact of recommending that parents who watch screens frequently limit their own SVT while their toddler is present. Future interventions should also take into account the evolution of screen usage, which tends to move from television to smartphones and tablets [40].

Abbreviations

AAP:

American Academy of Pediatrics

GUSTO:

Growing Up in Singapore Towards healthy Outcomes

SVT:

Screen viewing time

References

  1. Bureau of Labor Statistics, U.S. Department of Labor. American time use survey - 2013 results. Washington; 2014. https://www.bls.gov/news.release/archives/atus_06182014.pdf.

  2. Interactive Software Federation Of Europe. Videogames In Europe. Brussels: Consumer Study; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  3. NPD Group. Kids and gaming 2013. Port Washington, NY: The NPD Group, Inc.; 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  4. Verloigne M, Loyen A, Van Hecke L, Lakerveld J, Hendriksen I, De Bourdheaudhuij I, et al. Variation in population levels of sedentary time in European children and adolescents according to cross-European studies: a systematic literature review within DEDIPAC. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2016;13:69.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. World Health Organization: Global recommendations on physical activity for health. http://whqlibdoc.who.int/publications/2010/9789241599979_eng.pdf?ua=1; 2010.

  6. Lim SS, Vos T, Flaxman AD, Danaei G, Shibuya K, Adair-Rohani H, et al. A comparative risk assessment of burden of disease and injury attributable to 67 risk factors and risk factor clusters in 21 regions, 1990-2010: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2010. Lancet. 2012;380:2224–60.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  7. Lee IM, Shiroma EJ, Lobelo F, Puska P, Blair SN, Katzmarzyk PT, et al. Effect of physical inactivity on major non-communicable diseases worldwide: an analysis of burden of disease and life expectancy. Lancet. 2012;380:219–29.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  8. Sedentary Behaviour Research N. Letter to the editor: standardized use of the terms “sedentary” and “sedentary behaviours”. Appl Physiol Nutr Metab. 2012;37:540–2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. de Rezende LF, Rodrigues Lopes M, Rey-Lopez JP, Matsudo VK, Luiz OC. Sedentary behavior and health outcomes: an overview of systematic reviews. PLoS One. 2014;9:e105620.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Pate RR, Mitchell JA, Byun W, Dowda M. Sedentary behaviour in youth. Br J Sports Med. 2011;45:906–13.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Christakis DA, Garrison MM. Preschool-aged children's television viewing in child care settings. Pediatrics. 2009;124:1627–32.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Dennison BA, Erb TA, Jenkins PL. Television viewing and Television in Bedroom Associated with Overweight Risk among low-Income Preschool Children. Pediatrics. 2002;109:1028–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Reid Chassiakos Y, Radesky J, Christakis D, Moreno MA, Cross C. Children and adolescents and digital media. Pediatrics. 2016;138:e20162593-e.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Saldanha-Gomes C, Heude B, Charles MA, de Lauzon-Guillain B, Botton J, Carles S, et al. Prospective associations between energy balance-related behaviors at 2 years of age and subsequent adiposity: the EDEN mother-child cohort. Int J Obes. 2017;41:38–45.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  15. Council On Communications Media. Media and young minds. Pediatrics. 2016;138:e20162591-e.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. 2015 Google Consumer Barometer [https://www.consumerbarometer.com].

  17. Hildebrand M, Øglund GP, Wells JC, Ekelund U. Prenatal, birth and early life predictors of sedentary behavior in young people: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activ. 2016;13

  18. Duch H, Fisher EM, Ensari I, Harrington A. Screen time use in children under 3 years old: a systematic review of correlates. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2013;10:102.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  19. Goh SN, Teh LH, Tay WR, Anantharaman S, Dam RM, Tan CS, et al. Sociodemographic, home environment and parental influences on total and device-specific screen viewing in children aged 2 years and below: an observational study. BMJ Open. 2016;6:e009113.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  20. Soh SE, Tint MT, Gluckman PD, Godfrey KM, Rifkin-Graboi A, Chan YH, et al. Cohort profile: growing up in Singapore towards healthy outcomes (GUSTO) birth cohort study. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43:1401–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Padmapriya N, Shen L, Soh SE, Shen Z, Kwek K, Godfrey KM, et al. Physical activity and sedentary behavior patterns before and during pregnancy in a multi-ethnic sample of Asian women in Singapore. Matern Child Health J. 2015;19:2523–35.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Tu Y-K, Gilthorpe MS. Statistical thinking in epidemiology. Boca Raton: CRC Press; 2011.

  23. Biddle SJ, Pearson N, Ross GM, Braithwaite R. Tracking of sedentary behaviours of young people: a systematic review. Prev Med. 2010;51:345–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Certain LK, Kahn RS. Prevalence, correlates, and trajectory of television viewing among infants and toddlers. Pediatrics. 2002;109:634–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Flores G, Tomany-Korman SC, Olson L. Does disadvantage start at home? Racial and ethnic disparities in health-related early childhood home routines and safety practices. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2005;159:158–65.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Schmidt ME, Rich M, Rifas-Shiman SL, Oken E, Taveras EM. Television viewing in infancy and child cognition at 3 years of age in a US cohort. Pediatrics. 2009;123:e370–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Carson V, Janssen I. Associations between factors within the home setting and screen time among children aged 0-5 years: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:539.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Chandra M, Jalaludin B, Woolfenden S, Descallar J, Nicholls L, Dissanayake C, et al. Screen time of infants in Sydney. Australia: a birth cohort study BMJ Open. 2016;6:e012342.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Dong S, Song Y, Jiang Y, Sun W, Wang Y, Jiang F. Multi-center study on the effects of television viewing on sleep quality among children under 4 years of age in China. Zhonghua Er Ke Za Zhi. 2015;53:907–12.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Cui Z, Hardy LL, Dibley MJ, Bauman A. Temporal trends and recent correlates in sedentary behaviours in Chinese children. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2011;8:93.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Huang WY, Wong SH, Salmon J. Correlates of physical activity and screen-based behaviors in Chinese children. J Sci Med Sport. 2013;16:509–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Atkin AJ, Sharp SJ, Corder K, van Sluijs EM, International Children’s Accelerometry Database C. Prevalence and correlates of screen time in youth: an international perspective. Am J Prev Med. 2014;47:803–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Zimmerman FJ, Christakis DA, Meltzoff AN. Television and DVD/video viewing in children younger than 2 years. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007;161:473–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  34. Songul Yalcin S, Tugrul B, Nacar N, Tuncer M, Yurdakok K. Factors that affect television viewing time in preschool and primary schoolchildren. Pediatr Int. 2002;44:622–7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Tomopoulos S, Dreyer BP, Valdez P, Flynn V, Foley G, Berkule SB, et al. Media content and externalizing behaviors in Latino toddlers. Ambul Pediatr. 2007;7:232–8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Kourlaba G, Kondaki K, Liarigkovinos T, Manios Y. Factors associated with television viewing time in toddlers and preschoolers in Greece: the GENESIS study. J Public Health (Oxf). 2009;31:222–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Hoyos Cillero I, Jago R. Systematic review of correlates of screen-viewing among young children. Prev Med. 2010;51:3–10.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Gopinath B, Baur LA, Hardy LL, Kifley A, Rose KA, Wong TY, et al. Relationship between a range of sedentary behaviours and blood pressure during early adolescence. J Hum Hypertens. 2012;26:350–6.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Singapore Department of Statistics, Ministry of Trade and Industry. Census of population 2010 statistical release 1: demographic characteristics, education, language and religion, vol. 2011. http://www.singstat.gov.sg/publications/publications-and-papers/cop2010/census10_stat_release1.

  40. Hinkley T, Cliff DP, Okely AD. Reducing electronic media use in 2-3 year-old children: feasibility and efficacy of the family@play pilot randomised controlled trial. BMC Public Health. 2015;15:779.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the members of the GUSTO study group: Allan Sheppard, Amutha Chinnadurai, Anne Eng Neo Goh, Anne Rifkin-Graboi, Anqi Qiu, Arijit Biswas, Bee Wah Lee, Birit F.P. Broekman, Boon Long Quah, Borys Shuter, Chai Kiat Chng, Cheryl Ngo, Choon Looi Bong, Christiani Jeyakumar Henry, Cornelia Yin Ing Chee, Yam Thiam Daniel Goh, Doris Fok, Fabian Yap, George Seow Heong Yeo, Helen Chen, Hugo P S van Bever, Iliana Magiati, Inez Bik Yun Wong, Ivy Yee-Man Lau, Jeevesh Kapur, Jenny L. Richmond, Jerry Kok Yen Chan, Joanna D. Holbrook, Joshua J. Gooley, Keith M. Godfrey, Kenneth Kwek, Kok Hian Tan, Krishnamoorthy Niduvaje, Leher Singh, Lin Lin Su, Lourdes Mary Daniel, Lynette Pei-Chi Shek, Marielle V. Fortier, Mark Hanson, Mary Foong-Fong Chong, Mary Rauff, Mei Chien Chua, Michael Meaney, Mya Thway Tint, Neerja Karnani, Ngee Lek, Oon Hoe Teoh, P. C. Wong, Peter D. Gluckman, Pratibha Agarwal, Rob M. van Dam, Salome A. Rebello, Seang-Mei Saw, Shang Chee Chong, Shirong Cai, Shu-E Soh, Sok Bee Lim, Chin-Ying Stephen Hsu, Victor Samuel Rajadurai, Walter Stunkel, Wee Meng Han, Wei Wei Pang, Yap-Seng Chong, Yin Bun Cheung, Yiong Huak Chan and Yung Seng Lee.

Funding

This research was supported by the Singapore National Research Foundation under its Translational and Clinical Research (TCR) Flagship Programme and administered by the Singapore Ministry of Health’s National Medical Research Council (NMRC), Singapore- NMRC/TCR/004-NUS/2008; NMRC/TCR/012-NUHS/2014. Additional funding of the present study was provided by the Singapore Institute for Clinical Sciences, A*STAR and Nestec. The study sponsors were not involved in study design, data collection, analysis and interpretation of data, manuscript writing, and the decision to submit the article for publication. KMG is supported by the National Institute for Health Research through the NIHR Southampton Biomedical Research Centre and by the European Union’s Seventh Framework Programme (FP7/2007-2013), project EarlyNutrition under grant agreement number 289346.

Availability of data and materials

The dataset supporting the conclusions of this article can be made available upon request, and after approval by the GUSTO Executive Committee.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

KHT, FY, LS, YSC, PDG, KMG and SMS conceived and designed the cohort study; JYB, MSK and FMR designed the present work; NP and SC contributed to data collection; NP, BC, MSK and FMR contributed to data analysis and interpretation led by JYB; JYB and NP drafted the manuscript, and MSK and FMR added important intellectual content; all authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jonathan Y. Bernard.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All participants signed written informed consent at enrolment. The study received ethical approval from the National Healthcare Group Domain Specific Review Board and the SingHealth Centralised Institutional Review Board.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

LS, YSC and KMG have received reimbursement for speaking at conferences sponsored by companies selling nutritional products. They are part of an academic consortium that has received research funding from Abbott Nutrition, Nestle, and Danone. All other authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Additional files

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Summary of the measurement method and time point of assessment of the variables examined as predictors of screen viewing time in 2- and 3-year-old children from the GUSTO cohort study. (DOCX 12 kb)

Additional file 2: Figure S1.

Television and hand-held devices viewing time (h/day) at ages 2 and 3 years according to maternal age in children from the GUSTO cohort study. Values are means ± SE, and p-values are for the interaction term between maternal age and child age, with viewing time as outcome. (PNG 25 kb)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons license, and indicate if changes were made. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Bernard, J.Y., Padmapriya, N., Chen, B. et al. Predictors of screen viewing time in young Singaporean children: the GUSTO cohort. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 14, 112 (2017). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12966-017-0562-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/s12966-017-0562-3

Keywords