Skip to main content

Residential self-selection bias in the estimation of built environment effects on physical activity between adolescence and young adulthood

Abstract

Background

Built environment research is dominated by cross-sectional designs, which are particularly vulnerable to residential self-selection bias resulting from health-related attitudes, neighborhood preferences, or other unmeasured characteristics related to both neighborhood choice and health-related outcomes.

Methods

We used cohort data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (United States; Wave I, 1994-95; Wave III, 2001-02; n = 12,701) and a time-varying geographic information system. Longitudinal relationships between moderate to vigorous physical activity (MVPA) bouts and built and socioeconomic environment measures (landcover diversity, pay and public physical activity facilities per 10,000 population, street connectivity, median household income, and crime rate) from adolescence to young adulthood were estimated using random effects models (biased by unmeasured confounders) and fixed effects models (within-person estimator, which adjusts for unmeasured confounders that are stable over time).

Results

Random effects models yielded null associations except for negative crime-MVPA associations [coefficient (95% CI): -0.056 (-0.083, -0.029) in males, -0.061 (-0.090, -0.033) in females]. After controlling for measured and time invariant unmeasured characteristics using within-person estimators, MVPA was higher with greater physical activity pay facilities in males [coefficient (95% CI): 0.024 (0.006, 0.042)], and lower with higher crime rates in males [coefficient (95% CI): -0.107 (-0.140, -0.075)] and females [coefficient (95% CI): -0.046 (-0.083, -0.009)]. Other associations were null or in the counter-intuitive direction.

Conclusions

Comparison of within-person estimates to estimates unadjusted for unmeasured characteristics suggest that residential self-selection can bias associations toward the null, as opposed to its typical characterization as a positive confounder. Differential environment-MVPA associations by residential relocation suggest that studies examining changes following residential relocation may be vulnerable to selection bias. The authors discuss complexities of adjusting for residential self-selection and residential relocation, particularly during the adolescent to young adult transition.

Background

Built environment characteristics such as walkability [1, 2] and availability of recreation centers [3, 4] are associated with physical activity (PA) in a growing literature. However, existing research is dominated by cross-sectional studies, which are particularly vulnerable to residential self-selection bias resulting from unmeasured neighborhood selection factors related to built environment exposures and PA [5, 6]. Neighborhood selection factors may include preference for PA resources, which could affect neighborhood choice and PA level. Similarly, social and financial resources not only influence where individuals can afford to live but also shape perceived barriers to PA. Furthermore, traditional covariate adjustment cannot adequately control for neighborhood preferences and other residential selection factors that are difficult or impossible to measure.

Longitudinal designs can address residential self-selection bias by establishing temporality and controlling for unmeasured characteristics. In two key longitudinal studies [7, 8], investigators used "first difference" models to estimate the influence of urban form on travel behavior or obesity. First difference models and a similar method, "fixed effects" models, use within-person estimators to control for unmeasured characteristics that remain constant throughout the study period [6, 9] (e.g., genetics or resilient attitudes toward exercise) by analyzing variation in the exposure and outcome within person, over time. Within-person estimation is especially valuable when confounders are difficult to measure (e.g., residential selection factors), and is most appropriate for exposure-outcome relationships with short lag times [10] (e.g., theorized built environment influences on PA). Recent longitudinal studies [11–14] investigating built environment effects on PA do not use within-person estimation to control for unmeasured characteristics.

Furthermore, the few relevant existing studies which use within-person estimation [2, 7, 8, 15] examine changes in behavior or body weight related to changes in urban form resulting from residential relocation. However, the environment can change around stationary residents. Furthermore, residential relocation is often triggered by events such as marriage or employment changes [16], which may also influence health-related behaviors. Therefore, restricting to those who move residences may induce selection bias [17].

Our primary objective was to estimate within-person effects of time-varying, objectively measured built and socioeconomic environment characteristics on moderate to vigorous PA (MVPA) in a nationally representative sample. Secondary objectives were to (a) assess the influence of time invariant, unmeasured characteristics on environment-PA associations by comparing within-person estimates to naïve estimates which do not address unmeasured characteristics, and (b) explore selectivity related to residential relocation. This paper reports the results of these objectives, followed by a discussion of the complexities of adjusting for residential self-selection and residential relocation, particularly during the adolescent to young adult transition.

Methods

Study population and data sources

We used Wave I (1994-95) and III (2001-02) data from The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health), a cohort study of 20,745 adolescents representative of the U.S. school-based population in grades 7 to 12 (11-22 years of age) in 1994-95 followed into adulthood (18-26 years at Wave III). Add Health included a core sample plus subsamples of selected minority and other groupings collected under protocols approved by the Institutional Review Board at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. The survey design and sampling frame have been discussed elsewhere [18].

Using a geographic information system (GIS), we linked respondents' Wave I and III residential locations to community-level data theorized to influence obesity and obesity-related behaviors. Among respondents in the probability sample (nWave I = 18,924, nWave III = 14,322), residential locations were determined from geocoded home addresses with street-segment matches (nWave I = 15,480, nWave III = 12,263), global positioning system (GPS) measurements (nWave I = 2,966, nWave III = 1,148), ZIP/ZIP+4/ZIP+2 centroid match (nWave I = 205, nWave III = 647) and geocoded school location (nWave I = 243; not applicable in Wave III, n = 264 unmatched). Comparison of individual-level and environmental measures across location sources suggest that respondent locations identified with GPS or ZIP codes (compared to geocoded addresses) were located in rural areas. Such differences were expected because rural residents more often use Post Office Boxes or other addresses that cannot be geocoded; that is, multiple location sources allowed us to include such respondents, thereby minimizing selection bias. Residential locations were linked to attributes of circular areas of various radii surrounding each wave-specific respondent residence (Euclidean neighborhood buffer) and block group, tract, and county attributes from time-matched U.S. Census and other data (see Study variables, below), which were merged with individual-level Add Health interview responses.

Of 18,924 Wave I respondents in the probability sample, 6% refused participation and 19% could not be located or were unable to participate for other reasons, leaving 14,322 Wave III respondents. Exclusions included mobility disability (n = 87) or self-reported pregnancy at Wave I or III (n = 578) and Native Americans due to small sample size (n = 121). Of the remaining sample (n = 13,546), those missing individual-level variables (n = 266), environmental variables (n = 568), or both (n = 11) were excluded. Those excluded due to missing data (n = 845) were generally similar to the analytical sample (n = 12,701) with regard to Wave I and III individual sociodemographics, MVPA, and environmental variables. Exceptions included lower census tract-level median income and Wave III landscape diversity, and higher Wave III MVPA in excluded respondents (data not shown).

Study variables

GIS-derived environmental characteristics

Geographic Units

We used neighborhood buffer sizes (e.g., 1 or 3 k) based on research showing that MVPA was most strongly and consistently associated with street connectivity within smaller areas (1 k) and with PA facilities within larger areas (3 k) [19], consistent with theorized higher incentive to travel to PA facilities and engagement in street-based activities closer to home. We selected census tracts for census variables based on similar sensitivity analysis (unpublished data), while crime data were available only at the county level.

Built and socioeconomic environment measures

We selected built and socioeconomic environment measures shown to adequately represent multidimensional environmental constructs [20]. Table 1 presents variable descriptions, data sources, and geographic unit. Briefly, payand public PA facilities countswere obtained from Dun and Bradstreet, a dataset of U.S. businesses validated against a field-based census [21]. We then calculated PA facility availability (counts per 10,000 population). In contrast with raw counts or distance to facilities, such population-scaled measures may help to separate availability of facilities from density of development, which are independently related to behavior [20, 22].

Table 1 Built and socioeconomic environment source measures: data sources and variable descriptions1

Simpson's Diversity Index, an indicator of landscape diversityand complexity [23], was calculated using Fragstats software [24]. Alpha indexindicated the degree of street connectivity [25], which provides numerous, often more direct route options [26]. Socioeconomic environment measures included census tract-level median household incomeand county-level non-violent and violent crime rateper 100,000 population.

To account for slight inaccuracies in geocoded locations and inconsequential moves, residential relocation(mover vs. non-movers) was defined as > 1/4 mile Euclidean distance between Wave I and III residential locations.

Individual-level variables

Weekly frequency (bouts) of leisure-time MVPA (skating & cycling, exercise, and active sports) was ascertained at Waves I and III using a standard, interview administered activity recall based on questionnaires validated in other epidemiologic studies [27, 28]. The questionnaire included activities relevant to adolescents (11-22 years) at Wave I and was modified at Wave III (18-26 years) to include age-appropriate activities, so Wave III bouts were scaled for comparability with Wave I [29]. Semi-continuous MVPA was rounded to the nearest integer for appropriate modeling as a count variable.

Individual-level sociodemographic control variables included Wave I self-identified race (white, black, Asian, Hispanic), parent-reported annual household income and highest education attained (< high school, high school or GED, some college, ≥ college degree), and age at Wave I and III interviews. To account for regional differences in MVPA and neighborhood environments, we controlled for administratively determined U.S. region (West, Midwest, South, Northeast). Socioeconomic position in young adulthood involves a complex array of behaviors and achievements [30, 31] which are potential predictors of residential relocation, so we used parent income and education to indicate socioeconomic position in both waves.

Statistical analysis

Descriptive analysis

Individual-level and environment variables were compared by residential relocation status using adjusted Wald tests and design-based F-tests (95% confidence level) for continuous and categorical variables, respectively. Analyses were weighted for national representation and corrected for complex survey design using Stata 10.1 survey commands. To address skewness, we report median and interquartile range and performed statistical tests on natural-log transformed pay and public facility availability and median household income.

Regression analysis

Within-person effects of environment measures on MVPA bouts from adolescence (Wave I) to young adulthood (Wave III) were estimated using fixed effects Poisson regression (Objective 1). Fixed effects (versus first differences) accommodate our nonlinear dependent variable. By analyzing deviations of the outcome and exposures from person-specific means, fixed effects models remove person-specific error and are therefore not biased by time invariant unmeasured characteristics. As demonstrated elsewhere [6, 8, 32] and in additional file 1, appendix A, interpretation of the coefficients is unchanged from traditional regression models. In contrast, "random effects" estimates incorporate both between- and within-person variation and thus do not control for unmeasured characteristics that vary or remain constant over time (naïve estimation; Objective 2a) [33].

The Hausman specification test formally compared fixed and random effects estimates. All models were fit using the Stata 10.1 xtpoisson function [34], which provided comparable estimates but does not accommodate probability weights. Sample weighted, school cluster-corrected, within-person estimates obtained using an alternative method [32] were substantively similar, but comparable random effects estimates were not possible given the available software. Random effects models corrected for school-level clustering by including school indicator variables [35]; higher-level clustering is subsumed into between-person variation which does not influence fixed effects regression models.

The MVPA distribution was overdispersed (the standard deviation was larger than assumed by the Poisson distribution), but the conditional likelihood for the negative binomial distribution required for fixed effects models is problematic [32]. However, additional error terms in random and fixed effects models [36] and correction for school-level variation may help to address overdispersion by allowing for sources of variability not included in a standard Poisson model. Estimates from cross-sectional Poisson and negative binomial models are virtually identical.

Buffer-based environment measures were individual-level variables. While census tracts or counties could comprise a third level in multilevel analysis, they are not nested within schools, our primary sampling unit and more important source of clustering. Additionally, our data were sparse (average 8 and 2.3 respondents per census tract in Wave I and III, respectively) and unbalanced (1-275 and 1-95 respondents per census tract in Wave I and III, respectively), so multilevel analysis may have produced biased estimates [37]. Intraclass correlations for ln(MVPA) were minimal (0.03 in both Waves; ICC's are not definable for Poisson distributed outcomes).

Natural log transformations of environment measures linearized relationships with MVPA bouts in preliminary analysis. Because both the dependent and independent variables were logged, model coefficients were interpreted as elasticities, or the percent change in MVPA bouts predicted from a 1% change in the independent variable. Time invariant individual-level variables were included in random effects models but are not estimated in fixed effects models. Time varying age was included in both models. Sex interactions with each environmental variable were tested; for comparability, interaction terms were retained if significant (Wald p < 0.10) in the random or fixed effects model. Further interaction with residential relocation status (Objective 2b) in fixed effects models was examined by including significant (Wald p < 0.10; lower order terms were retained) two- and three-way interactions between residential relocation status, sex, and each environment measure. When one or more interactions were included in the model, group-specific associations were reported.

Results

Individual-level characteristics are presented in Table 2. 68.5% (SE 1.2%) of the analytical sample moved between Waves I and III (data not shown), and changes in environmental measures observed between Waves I and III (Table 3) provided sufficient variability for estimation of within-person effects, even for non-movers.

Table 2 Sociodemographic characteristics in adolescence (Wave I, 1994-95) and young adulthood (Wave III, 2001-02) [mean/% (SE)]1
Table 3 Baseline and changes in built and socioeconomic environment characteristics between adolescence (Wave I, 1994-95) and young adulthood (Wave III, 2001-02), by residential relocation status1

Within-person estimates indicated that with 1% greater pay facilities in the neighborhood, MVPA bouts were 0.024% higher in males; corresponding associations were negative but not significant in females (Table 4). MVPA was negatively associated with crime and, for females in fixed effects models, marginally with median household income. Landscape diversity, public facility availability, and alpha index were unrelated to MVPA.

Table 4 Random and within-person effect estimates of built and socioeconomic environment characteristics on MVPA between adolescence (Wave I, 1994-95) and young adulthood (Wave III, 2001-02)1

The Hausman specification test rejected the null hypotheses (p < 0.001) that there is no correlation between unexplained person-specific variation and the independent variables. That is, changes in estimates after controlling for time invariant, unmeasured characteristics by applying the within-person estimator were statistically significant. Compared to random effect estimates, within-person elasticities were larger for pay facility availability and, in males, almost two times larger for crime rate. In females, the within-person estimator attenuated negative random effects estimates for crime and reversed the association to the counter-intuitive direction (marginally significant) for median household income (Table 4).

Several associations varied by residential relocation status and sex (Table 5). Elasticities between MVPA bouts and crime were substantially larger in non-movers than movers, and landscape diversity was negatively associated with MVPA only in non-movers. Public facility availability was positively associated with MVPA in female movers only, with variation in magnitude and direction by sex- and relocation status. Model coefficients and p-values corresponding to Tables 4 and 5 are reported in additional file 2, appendix B.

Table 5 Variation in within-person effect estimates of built and socioeconomic environment characteristics on MVPA between adolescence (Wave I, 1994-95) and young adulthood (Wave III, 2001-02) by residential relocation status1

Discussion

We estimated longitudinal effects of built and socioeconomic environment characteristics on MVPA bouts in a prospective study of adolescents as they transition into young adulthood. To our knowledge, ours is the first study to examine built environment changes resulting from either residential relocation or changes around stationary residents. After adjusting for unmeasured time invariant characteristics, MVPA bouts were higher with greater availability of pay facilities in males, and lower with higher crime in males and females. Other associations were null or in the counter-intuitive direction. However, we discuss several methodological considerations in the following sections.

Built environment findings in the Add Health population

In contrast to relatively consistent cross-sectional associations between the built environment and PA in the extant literature [38, 39], many cross-sectional [40] and random effects associations were weak or null in the Add Health population. Possible methodological explanations for these differences include our buffer-based environment measures and complications related to broad geographic variation and measurement of complex environments [20]. In another longitudinal, national study, urban sprawl was weakly related to obesity [8]; however, we expected a stronger, more robust relationship with PA, a more proximal outcome. Additionally, theorized behavior-specific relationships [41] such as promotion of walking for transit by highly connected streets could not be examined with our total leisure-time MVPA measure. Of course, null associations may reflect a lack of causal effects. Ultimately, several naïve estimates (cross-sectional and random effects) were null or counterintuitive, so corresponding within-person estimates cannot be attributed solely to adjustment for unmeasured time invariant characteristics.

Residential self-selection bias: upward, downward, or more complex?

Residential self-selection is typically presented as a positive confounder which may create or magnify associations between the built environment and PA [5, 6, 42]. This characterization assumes that hypothesized built environment PA supports are: (1) preferred by or correlated with other neighborhood characteristics selected by people with higher PA (e.g., high performing schools), or (2) uncommon in areas selected by people with generally lower PA (e.g., lack of resources in affordable neighborhoods). These assumptions are supported by disproportionate allocation of recreation resources to more affluent neighborhoods [3, 43–45] and by attenuation of relationships between urban form and health-related outcomes by first difference models [8] and other adjustment methods [5, 46, 47].

However, some PA-promoting features may be less common in advantaged areas. For example, pay facilities may encourage PA but may be more common in commercial centers potentially selected less often by advantaged families (with higher PA levels). In this scenario, residential self-selection factors are negative confounders, consistent with stronger positive estimated within-person (versus random) effects of pay facilities on MVPA in males.

In contrast, within-person (versus random effects) estimates of higher crime effects on lower MVPA were attenuated in females, suggesting that self-selection factors related to crime may operate differently in females versus males. That is, crime and safety may play a stronger role in not only MVPA but also selection of a neighborhood in females than in males. Overall, these results suggest that residential self-selection may magnify or attenuate built environment-PA associations and involves multifaceted relationships among complex environments and sex-specific determinants of residential selection and PA.

Furthermore, concerns that selection of neighborhoods based on activity-related amenities can explain positive environment-PA associations [5] suggests positive confounding but not necessarily absence of causal effects. That is, selected amenities may help active individuals to maintain or increase their activity levels, formally defined as "effect in the treated" [48]. Alternatively, "effect in the untreated" would support placement of activity-related amenities in areas of greatest need. Investigation of heterogeneous effects may clarify the potential value of various built environment modification strategies.

Within-person estimators applied to a life transition period

Within-person estimators control for unmeasured characteristics that remain constant over time, a major strength for addressing residential selection factors, which are challenging, if not impossible, to measure accurately [6]. However, examination of neighborhood effects during the adolescence to young adulthood transition raises several complications:

Time varying characteristics

Within-person estimators do not control for unmeasured characteristics which change over time. Residential relocation is typically triggered by marriage, childbearing, employment opportunities [16], or other events which characterize the adolescent to young adulthood transition [49] and may lead to changes PA. Sedentary employment or intensive schooling in young adulthood may reduce PA levels, overwhelming any built environment effects on PA. Such events may also influence the type of neighborhood selected, thus comprising time varying, potentially unmeasured confounders.

Because these events are rare in adolescence, there was insufficient variability in Wave I for analysis as time varying measures. For example, magnification of negative crime-MVPA associations by within-person estimation in males could be explained by movement into urban centers (with higher crime) for employment, which may limit leisure time for PA. Employment may therefore be a time varying confounder which is unmeasured in our study.

Importantly, similar residential relocation triggers may occur throughout middle and later adulthood, with similar implications for bias if they are not sufficiently measured. Further, because residential self-selection may attenuate estimated relationships, null associations do not necessarily imply that bias has been fully addressed. Exploration and development of approaches for addressing time-varying characteristics that are unmeasured is clearly an important area for future work. Possible strategies include instrumental variables methods or other simultaneous equation strategies which model predictors of residential selection and neighborhood predictors of behavior or health in two or more stages [6].

Age-specific effects

Our longitudinal models assume constant causal effects between time points [10], a questionable assumption during periods of shifting PA determinants. However, differences in published cross-sectional associations between Wave I and III were not statistically significant [40]. Nevertheless, estimated causal effects in adolescents versus young adults should be further investigated using longitudinal data and innovative adjustment strategies.

Residential selection by parents

Residential location was likely determined by parents in Wave I but respondents in Wave III. Therefore, the source of unmeasured residential selection factors varied across waves and may contribute additional bias. However, previous neighborhood characteristics are the most powerful predictors of subsequent neighborhood characteristics [50, 51], suggesting that key unmeasured characteristics may remain constant and carry across generations.

Summary

Within-person estimation has limitations but is particularly relevant for capturing short-term effects theorized for behavioral outcomes such as PA [10] and is overall a valuable approach for addressing residential self-selection bias.

Restriction by residential relocation status: an additional source of bias?

Biases related to residential stability may be at least as strong as residential relocation: in the adolescent to young adulthood transition, individuals may remain in the parent's home for reasons (e.g., care for young children, unemployment, or attendance at a local college) associated with health behaviors (outcomes), and neighborhoods (exposures) change systematically (e.g., disadvantaged groups more often live in neighborhoods with less advantageous environment trajectories [51]). Thus, conditioning on residential relocation may induce selection bias.

Indeed, movers and non-movers differ with regard to individual characteristics in this and prior studies [52] and to estimated environment-MVPA associations. With the exception of public facilities, associations were weaker or equivalent in movers than non-movers, but these patterns could be reversed in adulthood when residential stability is the norm.

Differential associations could also reflect different sets of unmeasured factors that influence residential selection (in movers) versus changes in neighborhoods around stationary residents (non-movers). In the full sample, we expect residential selection factors to dominate because the majority of the sample moved between Waves I and III. However, distinguishing between selection bias and differential confounding is complex and requires future research using analytical methods such as marginal structural models that can address relocation status without inducing selection bias through covariate adjustment or stratification [17].

Strengths and limitations

Limitations of this study include the methodological concerns raised above. Additionally, our definition of residential relocation did not capture duration of residence and may have misclassified respondents who moved short distances or moved but returned to the same location by Wave III. Second, changes in socioeconomic environment variables around a given location may reflect shifts in census boundaries between 1990 and 2000. Also, there was temporal mismatch between interview data and census and street connectivity data; in particular, temporal mismatch in Wave I was a tradeoff for greater accuracy of a more current street database. Third, neighborhood buffers delineated by street network distance may yield different results; however, population counts needed for our facility availability measures were not available within network buffer areas, and environment measures are similar for Euclidean versus network distance-based buffers. Additionally, conversion of population within buffers from population within block groups (Table 1) may have resulted in measurement error in our facilities availability measures and bias of unpredictable direction and magnitude in corresponding associations with MVPA, particularly in heterogenous areas. Fourth, our data sources may have captured relevant neighborhood characteristics more completely in some subgroups (e.g., our database does not capture PA resources on college campuses), potentially resulting in differential measurement error by study wave or sociodemographic group. Fifth, the PA environments at school, workplace, or other locations were not addressed in this study.

Loss to follow-up and missing individual-level data could have led to biased estimates. Our leisure time MVPA frequency measure does not distinguish between possible behavior-specific effects [41] (e.g. promotion of active transit versus exercise); incorporate physical activity duration or intensity; and may have systematically omitted important activities which could account for the observed sex differences. Also, while our Wave I MVPA measure was based on instruments validated in other epidemiologic child and adolescent studies, modifications made for Wave III (addition of age-appropriate activities) has not been validated in young adults. However, these are tradeoffs for the size and scope of the Add Health study. Finally, the direction of effect remains ambiguous, as we examined simultaneous changes in the environment and in MVPA bouts.

However, our unique time-varying environment database captures residential locations of a large, nationally representative population followed through a critical life stage. By including six built and socioeconomic environment measures shown to adequately represent key environmental constructs, we addressed environmental confounders while avoiding collinearity. Our longitudinal data was used to address residential self-selection bias and explore bias related to residential relocation.

Conclusions

After controlling for residential self-selection bias using within-person estimators, MVPA bouts were related only to pay facility availability in males and crime in males and females in the expected directions. Our results suggest that the magnitude and direction of residential self-selection bias can vary across environmental and individual characteristics. Within-person estimators are valuable for controlling for residential self-selection bias, but their application to the adolescence to young adulthood transition or other major life transitions is complex. Further research and development of methods that can address predictors of residential relocation while simultaneously controlling for unobserved measures is needed.

References

  1. Frank L, Kerr J, Chapman J, Sallis J: Urban form relationships with walk trip frequency and distance among youth. Am J Health Promot. 2007, 21: 305-311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  2. Ewing R, Brownson RC, Berrigan D: Relationship between urban sprawl and weight of United States youth. Am J Prev Med. 2006, 31: 464-474. 10.1016/j.amepre.2006.08.020.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Gordon-Larsen P, Nelson MC, Page P, Popkin BM: Inequality in the built environment underlies key health disparities in physical activity and obesity. Pediatrics. 2006, 117: 417-424. 10.1542/peds.2005-0058.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  4. Diez Roux AV, Evenson KR, McGinn AP, Brown DG, Moore L, Brines S, Jacobs DR: Availability of recreational resources and physical activity in adults. Am J Public Health. 2007, 97: 493-499. 10.2105/AJPH.2006.087734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  5. Mokhtarian PL, Cao X: Examining the impacts of residential selection on travel behavior: a focus on methodologies. Trans Research Part B. 2008, 42: 204-228. 10.1016/j.trb.2007.07.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  6. Boone-Heinonen J, Gordon-Larsen P, Guilkey D, Jacobs DR, Popkin BM: Environment and physical activity dynamics: the role of residential self-selection. Psychology of Sport and Exercise.

  7. Krizek KJ: Residential relocation and changes in urban travel. J Am Plan Assn. 2003, 69: 265-281. 10.1080/01944360308978019.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Eid J, Overman HG, Puga D, Turner MA: Fat city: Questioning the relationship between urban sprawl and obesity. Journal of Urban Economics. 2008, 63: 385-404. 10.1016/j.jue.2007.12.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  9. Do DP, Finch BK: The link between neighborhood poverty and health: Context or composition?. Am J Epidemiol. 2008, 168: 611-619. 10.1093/aje/kwn182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Glymour MM: Sensitive periods and first difference models: integrating etiologic thinking into econometric techniques: a commentary on Clarkwest's "Neo-materialist theory and the temporal relationship between income inequality and longevity change". Soc Sci Med. 2008, 66: 1895-1902. 10.1016/j.socscimed.2007.12.035. discussion 1903-1898

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Coogan PF, White LF, Adler TJ, Hathaway KM, Palmer JR, Rosenberg L: Prospective study of urban form and physical activity in the Black Women's Health Study. Am J Epidemiol. 2009, 170: 1105-1117. 10.1093/aje/kwp264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  12. Lee IM, Ewing R, Sesso HD: The built environment and physical activity levels: the Harvard Alumni Health Study. Am J Prev Med. 2009, 37: 293-298. 10.1016/j.amepre.2009.06.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Berry TR, Spence JC, Blanchard CM, Cutumisu N, Edwards J, Selfridge G: A longitudinal and cross-sectional examination of the relationship between reasons for choosing a neighbourhood, physical activity and body mass index. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 7: 57-10.1186/1479-5868-7-57.

  14. Hou N, Popkin BM, Jacobs DR, Song Y, Guilkey D, Lewis CE, Gordon-Larsen P: Longitudinal associations between neighborhood-level street network with walking, bicycling, and jogging: The CARDIA study. Health Place.

  15. Plantinga AJ, Bernell S: The association between urban sprawl and obesity: Is it a two-way street?. Journal of Regional Science. 2007, 47: 857-879. 10.1111/j.1467-9787.2007.00533.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Geist C, McManus PA: Geographical mobility over the life course: Motivations and implications. Population Space and Place. 2008, 14: 283-303. 10.1002/psp.508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Hernan MA, Hernandez-Diaz S, Robins JM: A structural approach to selection bias. Epidemiology. 2004, 15: 615-625. 10.1097/01.ede.0000135174.63482.43.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Resnick MD, Bearman PS, Blum RW, Bauman KE, Harris KM, Jones J, Tabor J, Beuhring T, Sieving RE, Shew M, et al: Protecting adolescents from harm. Findings from the National Longitudinal Study on Adolescent Health. JAMA. 1997, 278: 823-832. 10.1001/jama.278.10.823.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Boone-Heinonen J, Gordon-Larsen P, Song Y, Popkin BM: What neighborhood area captures built environment features related to adolescent physical activity?. Health Place.

  20. Boone-Heinonen J, Evenson KR, Song Y, Gordon-Larsen P: Built and socioeconomic environments: patterning and associations with physical activity in U.S. adolescents. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2010, 7: 45-10.1186/1479-5868-7-45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  21. Boone JE, Gordon-Larsen P, Stewart JD, Popkin BM: Validation of a GIS facilities database: quantification and implications of error. Ann Epidemiol. 2008, 18: 371-377. 10.1016/j.annepidem.2007.11.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. Cervero R, Kockelman K: Travel demand and the 3Ds: Density, diversity, and design. Transportation Research Part D-Transport and Environment. 1997, 2: 199-219. 10.1016/S1361-9209(97)00009-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Clifton K, Ewing R, Knaap GJ, Song Y: Quantitative analysis of urban form: a multidisciplinary review. J Urbanism. 2008, 1: 17-45.

    Google Scholar 

  24. McGarigal K, Cushman SA, Neel MC, Ene E: FRAGSTATS: Spatial Pattern Analysis Program for Categorical Maps. Book FRAGSTATS: Spatial Pattern Analysis Program for Categorical Maps (Editor ed.^eds.). 2002, City: University of Massachusetts, Amherst, [http://www.umass.edu/landeco/research/fragstats/fragstats.html]

    Google Scholar 

  25. Rodrigue J-P, Comtois C, Slack B: The Geography of Transport Systems. 2006, New York: Routledge

    Google Scholar 

  26. Saelens BE, Sallis JF, Frank LD: Environmental correlates of walking and cycling: findings from the transportation, urban design, and planning literatures. Ann Behav Med. 2003, 25: 80-91. 10.1207/S15324796ABM2502_03.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Sallis JF, Strikmiller PK, Harsha DW, Feldman HA, Ehlinger S, Stone EJ, Williston J, Woods S: Validation of interviewer- and self-administered physical activity checklists for fifth grade students. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1996, 28: 840-851.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  28. Prochaska JJ, Sallis JF, Long B: A physical activity screening measure for use with adolescents in primary care. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2001, 155: 554-559.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Willett WC: Nutritional Epidemiology. 1998, New York: Oxford University Press, 2

    Book  Google Scholar 

  30. Scharoun-Lee M, Adair LS, Kaufman JS, Gordon-Larsen P: Obesity, race/ethnicity and the multiple dimensions of socioeconomic status during the transition to adulthood: A factor analysis approach. Soc Sci Med. 2009

    Google Scholar 

  31. Scharoun-Lee M, Kaufman JS, Popkin BM, Gordon-Larsen P: Obesity, race/ethnicity and life course socioeconomic status across the transition from adolescence to adulthood. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2009, 63: 133-139. 10.1136/jech.2008.075721.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  32. Allison PD: Fixed Effects Regression Methods for Longitudinal Data Using SAS. 2005, Cary, NC: SAS Institute, Inc

    Google Scholar 

  33. Cameron AC, Trivedi PK: Microeconomics: Methods and Applications. 2005, New York: Cambridge University Press

    Book  Google Scholar 

  34. StataCorp: Longitudinal Panel Data manual, Stata Statistical Software, Release 9. 2005, College Station, TX: StataCorp LP

    Google Scholar 

  35. Angeles G, Guilkey DK, Mroz TA: The impact of community-level variables on individual-level - Outcomes theoretical results and applications. Sociological Methods & Research. 2005, 34: 76-121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Rabe-Hesketh S, Skrondal A: Multilevel and Longitudinal Modeling Using Stata. 2008, Stata Press, 2

    Google Scholar 

  37. Clarke P: When can group level clustering be ignored? Multilevel models versus single-level models with sparse data. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2008, 62: 752-758. 10.1136/jech.2007.060798.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  38. Saelens BE, Handy SL: Built environment correlates of walking: a review. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2008, 40: S550-566. 10.1249/MSS.0b013e31817c67a4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  39. Wendel-Vos W, Droomers M, Kremers S, Brug J, van Lenthe F: Potential environmental determinants of physical activity in adults: a systematic review. Obes Rev. 2007, 8: 425-440. 10.1111/j.1467-789X.2007.00370.x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  40. Boone-Heinonen J, Gordon-Larsen P: Age group- and sex-specificity in relationships between the built and socioeconomic environments and physical activity. J Epidemiol Community Health.

  41. Giles-Corti B, Timperio A, Bull F, Pikora T: Understanding physical activity environmental correlates: increased specificity for ecological models. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2005, 33: 175-181. 10.1097/00003677-200510000-00005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Bhat CR, Guo JY: A comprehensive analysis of built environment characteristics on household residential choice and auto ownership levels. Transportation Research Part B-Methodological. 2007, 41: 506-526. 10.1016/j.trb.2005.12.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Estabrooks PA, Lee RE, Gyurcsik NC: Resources for physical activity participation: does availability and accessibility differ by neighborhood socioeconomic status?. Ann Behav Med. 2003, 25: 100-104. 10.1207/S15324796ABM2502_05.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Moore LV, Diez Roux AV, Evenson KR, McGinn AP, Brines SJ: Availability of recreational resources in minority and low socioeconomic status areas. Am J Prev Med. 2008, 34: 16-22. 10.1016/j.amepre.2007.09.021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Powell LM, Slater S, Chaloupka FJ, Harper D: Availability of physical activity-related facilities and neighborhood demographic and socioeconomic characteristics: a national study. Am J Public Health. 2006, 96: 1676-1680. 10.2105/AJPH.2005.065573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Schwanen T, Mokhtarian PL: What if you live in the wrong neighborhood? The impact of residential neighborhood type dissonance on distance traveled. Transportation Research Part D. 2005, 10: 127-151. 10.1016/j.trd.2004.11.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  47. Cao X, Mokhtarian PL, Handy SL: Do changes in neighborhood characteristics lead to changes in travel behavior ? A structural equations modeling approach. 2007, 535-556.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Morgan SL, Winship C: The counterfactual model. Counterfactuals and causal inference. 2007, New York: Cambridge University Press

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  49. Park MJ, Mulye TP, Adams SH, Brindis CD, Irwin CE: The health status of young adults in the United States. Journal of Adolescent Health. 2006, 39: 305-317. 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2006.04.017.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Clark WAV, Ledwith V: How much does income matter in neighborhood choice?. Population Research and Policy Review. 2007, 26: 145-161. 10.1007/s11113-007-9026-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Sampson RJ, Sharkey P: Neighborhood selection and the social reproduction of concentrated racial inequality. Demography. 2008, 45: 1-29. 10.1353/dem.2008.0012.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  52. van Lenthe FJ, Martikainen P, Mackenbach JP: Neighbourhood inequalities in health and health-related behaviour: results of selective migration?. Health Place. 2007, 13: 123-137. 10.1016/j.healthplace.2005.09.013.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development at the National Institutes of Health (R01-HD057194 and R01-HD041375, R01-HD39183); a cooperative agreement with the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC SIP No. 5-00); and grants from the Robert Wood Johnson Foundation's Active Living Research and Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (R36-EH000380) and The Henry Dearman and Martha Stucker Dissertation Fellowship in the Royster Society of Fellows at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. There were no potential or real conflicts of financial or personal interest with the financial sponsors of the scientific project. The financial sponsors had no role in the study (design, data collection, analysis, interpretation, writing, or decision to submit the manuscript for publication).

This research uses data from Add Health, a program project designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris, and funded by a grant P01-HD31921 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 17 other agencies. Special acknowledgment is due Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for assistance in the original design. Persons interested in obtaining data files from Add Health should contact Add Health, CPC, 123 W. Franklin Street, Chapel Hill, NC 27516-2524 addhealth@unc.edu. No direct support was received from grant P01-HD31921 for this analysis.

The authors would like to thank Brian Frizzelle, Marc Peterson, Chris Mankoff, James D. Stewart, Phil Bardsley, and Diane Kaczor of the University of North Carolina, Carolina Population Center (CPC) and the CPC Spatial Analysis Unit for creation of the environmental variables. The authors also thank Drs. Barry M. Popkin, Linda S. Adair, and Yan Song for their critical review of the manuscript and Ms. Frances Dancy for her helpful administrative assistance.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Penny Gordon-Larsen.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors' contributions

JB-H designed the study, performed statistical analyses, and drafted the manuscript. DKG participated in study design, provided statistical consultation, and critically reviewed the manuscript. KRE provided study design feedback and critically reviewed the manuscript. PG-L supervised all aspects of the study. All authors read and approved the manuscript.

Electronic supplementary material

12966_2010_376_MOESM1_ESM.DOC

Additional file 1: Appendix A, Unmeasured variables in fixed effects models. Detailed description of fixed effects models and how they control for time constant unmeasured variables. (DOC 34 KB)

12966_2010_376_MOESM2_ESM.DOC

Additional file 2: Appendix B, Supplemental tables. Model coefficients and p-values for main effects and interaction terms (Tables B1 and B2) corresponding to effect estimates reported in Tables 4 and 5. (DOC 49 KB)

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an Open Access article is distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License ( https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0 ), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Boone-Heinonen, J., Guilkey, D.K., Evenson, K.R. et al. Residential self-selection bias in the estimation of built environment effects on physical activity between adolescence and young adulthood. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 7, 70 (2010). https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1479-5868-7-70

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://0-doi-org.brum.beds.ac.uk/10.1186/1479-5868-7-70

Keywords